Applying a Health Behavior Theory to Explore the Influence of Information and Experience on Arsenic Risk Representations, Policy Beliefs, and Protective Behavior

The common sense model (CSM) shows how people process information to construct representations, or mental models, that guide responses to health threats. We applied the CSM to understand how people responded to information about arsenic-contaminated well water. Constructs included external information (arsenic level and information use), experience (perceived water quality and arsenic-related health effects), representations, safety judgments, opinions about policies to mitigate environmental arsenic, and protective behavior. Of 649 surveys mailed to private well users with arsenic levels exceeding the maximum contaminant level, 545 (84%) were analyzed. Structural equation modeling quantified CSM relationships. Both external information and experience had substantial effects on behavior. Participants who identified a water problem were more likely to reduce exposure to arsenic. However, about 60% perceived good water quality and 60% safe water. Participants with higher arsenic levels selected higher personal safety thresholds and 20% reported a lower arsenic level than indicated by their well test. These beliefs would support judgments of safe water. A variety of psychological and contextual factors may explain judgments of safe water when information suggested otherwise. Information use had an indirect effect on policy beliefs through understanding environmental causes of arsenic. People need concrete information about environmental risk at both personal and environmental-systems levels to promote a comprehensive understanding and response. The CSM explained responses to arsenic information and may have application to other environmental risks.

[1]  M. Siegrist,et al.  Perception of Hazards: The Role of Social Trust and Knowledge , 2000, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[2]  Gregory L. Poe,et al.  Do Participants in Well Water Testing Programs Update Their Exposure and Health Risk Perceptions , 1996 .

[3]  B. Johnson Risk Communication: A Mental Models Approach , 2002 .

[4]  S. Dunwoody,et al.  Proposed model of the relationship of risk information seeking and processing to the development of preventive behaviors. , 1999, Environmental research.

[5]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  P. Chisnall Mail and Internet Surveys: The Tailored Design Method , 2007, Journal of Advertising Research.

[7]  P. Patrician Multiple imputation for missing data. , 2002, Research in nursing & health.

[8]  M. A. Safer,et al.  The Impact of Communications on the Self-Regulation of Health Beliefs, Decisions, and Behavior , 1983, Health education quarterly.

[9]  S. Dunwoody,et al.  The Relation of Communication to Risk Judgment and Preventive Behavior Related to Lead in Tap Water , 2000, Health communication.

[10]  Youjae Yi,et al.  Assumptions of the Two-Step Approach to Latent Variable Modeling , 1992 .

[11]  L. Cameron,et al.  Illness representations and matching labels with symptoms. , 1989, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[12]  J. Michela Causal Attributions in Health and Illness , 1986 .

[13]  Baruch Fischhoff,et al.  Characterizing Mental Models of Hazardous Processes: A Methodology and an Application to Radon , 1992 .

[14]  N. Weinstein What does it mean to understand a risk? Evaluating risk comprehension. , 1999, Journal of the National Cancer Institute. Monographs.

[15]  Keith Petrie,et al.  The Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R) , 2002 .

[16]  Kathleen Shordt,et al.  Arsenic in drinking water. , 2001, Journal of environmental health.

[17]  T. Rossman,et al.  Evidence that arsenite acts as a cocarcinogen in skin cancer. , 2004, Toxicology and applied pharmacology.

[18]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Pain and pleasure: Alternative interpretations for identical stimulation , 1980 .

[19]  E. Vaughan Chronic exposure to an environmental hazard: risk perceptions and self-protective behavior. , 1993, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[20]  Rex B. Kline,et al.  Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling , 1998 .

[21]  S. Dunwoody Scientists, journalists, and the meaning of uncertainty. , 1999 .

[22]  Roger L. Brown Assessing specific mediational effects in complex theoretical models , 1997 .

[23]  Rocky Ross,et al.  Mental models , 2004, SIGA.

[24]  S. Epstein Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. , 1994, The American psychologist.

[25]  An experiment in common sense: education at blood pressure screening. , 1989, Patient education and counseling.

[26]  Oene Wiegman,et al.  Verification of Information Through Direct Experiences With an Industrial Hazard , 1991 .

[27]  James C. Anderson,et al.  STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING IN PRACTICE: A REVIEW AND RECOMMENDED TWO-STEP APPROACH , 1988 .

[28]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample , 1987, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[29]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  Emotional memory: The effects of stress on "cool" and "hot" memory systems. , 1998 .

[30]  D. Wiebe,et al.  Defensive denial, affect, and the self-regulation of health threats. , 2003 .

[31]  P D Mullen,et al.  A meta-analysis of trials evaluating patient education and counseling for three groups of preventive health behaviors. , 1997, Patient education and counseling.

[32]  N. Weinstein,et al.  Predicting Homeowners' Mitigation Responses to Radon Test Data , 1992 .

[33]  Takahiko Yoshida,et al.  Chronic health effects in people exposed to arsenic via the drinking water: dose-response relationships in review. , 2004, Toxicology and applied pharmacology.

[34]  R. Zimmerman,et al.  Adverse Health Experiences, Environmental Attitudes, and Pesticide Usage Behavior of Farm Operators , 1999, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[35]  P. Levallois,et al.  Evaluation of consumer attitudes on taste and tap water alternatives in Québec , 1999 .

[36]  W. Klein,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Resistance of Personal Risk Perceptions to Debiasing Interventions , 2002 .

[37]  Martin S. Hagger,et al.  A Meta-Analytic Review of the Common-Sense Model of Illness Representations , 2003 .

[38]  Lucy Johnston,et al.  Resisting change: Information-seeking and stereotype change. , 1996 .

[39]  Melissa L. Finucane,et al.  Risk as Analysis and Risk as Feelings: Some Thoughts about Affect, Reason, Risk, and Rationality , 2004, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[40]  K Witte,et al.  Predicting risk behaviors: development and validation of a diagnostic scale. , 1996, Journal of health communication.

[41]  Howard Leventhal,et al.  The common-sense model of self-regulation of health and illness. , 2003 .

[42]  M. Browne,et al.  Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit , 1992 .

[43]  P. Slovic Trust, Emotion, Sex, Politics, and Science: Surveying the Risk‐Assessment Battlefield , 1999, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[44]  Peter M. Bentler,et al.  Practical Issues in Structural Modeling , 1987 .