Updating target distance across eye movements in depth.

We tested between two coding mechanisms that the brain may use to retain distance information about a target for a reaching movement across vergence eye movements. If the brain was to encode a retinal disparity representation (retinal model), i.e., target depth relative to the plane of fixation, each vergence eye movement would require an active update of this representation to preserve depth constancy. Alternatively, if the brain was to store an egocentric distance representation of the target by integrating retinal disparity and vergence signals at the moment of target presentation, this representation should remain stable across subsequent vergence shifts (nonretinal model). We tested between these schemes by measuring errors of human reaching movements (n = 14 subjects) to remembered targets, briefly presented before a vergence eye movement. For comparison, we also tested their directional accuracy across version eye movements. With intervening vergence shifts, the memory-guided reaches showed an error pattern that was based on the new eye position and on the depth of the remembered target relative to that position. This suggests that target depth is recomputed after the gaze shift, supporting the retinal model. Our results also confirm earlier literature showing retinal updating of target direction. Furthermore, regression analyses revealed updating gains close to one for both target depth and direction, suggesting that the errors arise after the updating stage during the subsequent reference frame transformations that are involved in reaching.

[1]  D. Sparks,et al.  Spatial localization of saccade targets. I. Compensation for stimulation-induced perturbations in eye position. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  T. Mergner,et al.  Visual object localisation in space , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  W Pieter Medendorp,et al.  Rotational Remapping in Human Spatial Memory during Eye and Head Motion , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  W Pieter Medendorp,et al.  Gaze-centered updating of remembered visual space during active whole-body translations. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  J. Crawford,et al.  Gaze-Centered Remapping of Remembered Visual Space in an Open-Loop Pointing Task , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  G C DeAngelis,et al.  The physiology of stereopsis. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  K. P. Krommenhoek,et al.  Evidence for nonretinal feedback in combined version-vergence eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  H. Sakata,et al.  The TINS Lecture The parietal association cortex in depth perception and visual control of hand action , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[9]  P. Viviani,et al.  Error parsing in visuomotor pointing reveals independent processing of amplitude and direction. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  W Pieter Medendorp,et al.  Behavioral reference frames for planning human reaching movements. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Aldo Genovesio,et al.  Integration of retinal disparity and fixation-distance related signals toward an egocentric coding of distance in the posterior parietal cortex of primates. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Disparity sensitivity of frontal eye field neurons. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Why Does the Brain Predict Sensory Consequences of Oculomotor Commands? Optimal Integration of the Predicted and the Actual Sensory Feedback , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  P. Medendorp,et al.  Visuospatial updating of reaching targets in near and far space , 2002, Neuroreport.

[15]  M. Loda,et al.  Coding of smooth eye movements in three-dimensional space by frontal cortex , 2022 .

[16]  C. Rivadulla,et al.  Depth Perception in Random Dot Stereograms Is Not Affected by Changes in Either Vergence or Accommodation , 1998, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[17]  Lawrence H Snyder,et al.  Spatial memory following shifts of gaze. I. Saccades to memorized world-fixed and gaze-fixed targets. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  G. DeAngelis Seeing in three dimensions: the neurophysiology of stereopsis , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  J. Philbeck,et al.  Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions , 1997 .

[20]  Nuo Li,et al.  Updating Visual Space during Motion in Depth , 2005, Neuron.

[21]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[22]  W Pieter Medendorp,et al.  Motion Parallax Is Computed in the Updating of Human Spatial Memory , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Do Visual Cues Contribute to the Neural Estimate of Viewing Distance Used by the Oculomotor System? , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Marcus Kaiser,et al.  Perisaccadic Mislocalization Orthogonal to Saccade Direction , 2004, Neuron.

[25]  R. Wurtz,et al.  A Pathway in Primate Brain for Internal Monitoring of Movements , 2002, Science.

[26]  T. Vilis,et al.  Gaze-Centered Updating of Visual Space in Human Parietal Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  A. Dobbins,et al.  Distance modulation of neural activity in the visual cortex. , 1998, Science.

[29]  John M. Allman,et al.  The Effect of Gaze Angle and Fixation Distance on the Responses of Neurons in V1, V2, and V4 , 2002, Neuron.

[30]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Updating of the visual representation in monkey striate and extrastriate cortex during saccades , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  P Viviani,et al.  Pointing to Kinesthetic Targets in Space , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  J W Gnadt,et al.  Eye movements in depth: What does the monkey's parietal cortex tell the superior colliculus? , 1998, Neuroreport.

[34]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[35]  D. Y. P. Henriques,et al.  Direction-dependent distortions of retinocentric space in the visuomotor transformation for pointing , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  W P Medendorp,et al.  Visuospatial memory computations during whole-body rotations in roll. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Estimation of passive horizontal linear whole-body displacement in humans. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  O. Bock,et al.  Contribution of retinal versus extraretinal signals towards visual localization in goal-directed movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Binocular Vision and Stereopsis , 1996 .

[40]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[41]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Roles of gravitational cues and efference copy signals in the rotational updating of memory saccades. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  J. Philbeck,et al.  Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Saccades can be aimed at the spatial location of targets flashed during pursuit. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. III. Maintenance of a spatially accurate saccade signal. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  C. Genovese,et al.  Remapping in human visual cortex. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  A P Batista,et al.  Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. , 1999, Science.

[47]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neurons in the monkey superior colliculus predict the visual result of impending saccadic eye movements. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  C. Genovese,et al.  Spatial Updating in Human Parietal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[49]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Impairment of gaze-centered updating of reach targets in bilateral parietal-occipital damaged patients. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  The Computational Neurobiology of Reaching and Pointing: A Foundation for Motor Learning , 2004 .

[51]  C. Gielen,et al.  Geometric computations underlying eye-hand coordination: orientations of the two eyes and the head , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  W. P. Medendorp,et al.  Pointing to remembered visual targets after active one-step self-displacements within reaching space , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  Emiliano Brunamonti,et al.  Postsaccadic Activities in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of Primates Are Influenced by both Eye Movement Vectors and Eye Position , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Comparison of cortico-cortical and cortico-collicular signals for the generation of saccadic eye movements. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  B. Julesz Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .

[56]  A. V. van den Berg,et al.  Localization of the plane of regard in space , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  L E Mays,et al.  Neurons in monkey parietal area LIP are tuned for eye-movement parameters in three-dimensional space. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered by prior saccades , 1976, Vision Research.