Probability Cuing of Target Location Facilitates Visual Search Implicitly in Normal Participants and Patients with Hemispatial Neglect
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Kenneth M. Heilman,et al. Hemispace and Hemispatial Neglect , 1987 .
[2] Hiroshi Tsukagoshi,et al. Eye-fixation patterns in homonymous hemianopia and unilateral spatial neglect , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[3] D. Robinson,et al. Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements , 1996, Nature.
[4] M. Kinsbourne,et al. Effects of left-sided movements on line bisection in unilateral neglect , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[5] Gregory S. Berns,et al. The Context of Uncertainty Modulates the Subcortical Response to Predictability , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[6] F. Sengpiel,et al. Visual perception: Spotlight on the primary visual cortex , 1999, Current Biology.
[7] M. Chun,et al. Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.
[8] G R Mangun,et al. Combined expectancies: event-related potentials reveal the early benefits of spatial attention that are obscured by reaction time measures. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] W. Baum,et al. Matching, undermatching, and overmatching in studies of choice. , 1979, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[10] G. Humphreys,et al. BIASED ATTENTIONAL SHIFTS ASSOCIATED WITH UNILATERAL LEFT NEGLECT , 2000, Cognitive Neuropsychology.
[11] Marlene Behrmann,et al. Hemispatial Neglect and Visual Search: A Large Scale Analysis , 2004, Cortex.
[12] J. Marshall,et al. Hemispheric activation vs spatio-motor cueing in visual neglect: A case study , 1991, Neuropsychologia.
[13] G. Fink,et al. Neural consequences of competing stimuli in both visual hemifields: A physiological basis for visual extinction , 2000, Annals of neurology.
[14] J. Wolfe,et al. Attention is fast but volition is slow , 2000, Nature.
[15] U. Greggers,et al. Matching behavior of honeybees in a multiple-choice situation: The differential effect of environmental stimuli on the choice process , 1997 .
[16] R J HERRNSTEIN,et al. Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. , 1961, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[17] U. Mayr,et al. Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[18] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. The effect of cueing on unilateral neglect , 1983, Neuropsychologia.
[19] M. Mesulam. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[20] H. Hoffman,et al. Unconscious acquisition of complex procedural knowledge. , 1987 .
[21] S. Mizumori,et al. Neurons in rat medial prefrontal cortex show anticipatory rate changes to predictable differential rewards in a spatial memory task , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.
[22] M. Corbetta,et al. A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.
[23] T. Curran,et al. Effects of aging on visuospatial attention: an ERP study , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[24] W. Milberg,et al. Preattentive and attentive visual search in individuals with hemispatial neglect. , 2000, Neuropsychology.
[25] Lynn C. Robertson,et al. Visual Search Performance in the Neglect Syndrome , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[26] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] M Behrmann,et al. Attention accesses multiple reference frames: evidence from visual neglect. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] M. Corbetta,et al. Influence of Stimulus Salience and Attentional Demands on Visual Search Patterns in Hemispatial Neglect , 1997, Brain and Cognition.
[29] Georg Kerkhoff,et al. Spatial hemineglect in humans , 2001, Progress in Neurobiology.
[30] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Location-specific target expectancies in visual search , 1999 .
[31] S. Ferber,et al. Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe , 2001, Nature.
[32] R. Andersen,et al. Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.
[33] S. E. Black,et al. Impaired visual search in patients with unilateral neglect: an oculographic analysis , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[34] David E. Irwin,et al. Visual Search has Memory , 2001, Psychological science.
[35] M. A. Basso,et al. Modulation of Neuronal Activity in Superior Colliculus by Changes in Target Probability , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] Matthew Flatt,et al. PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .
[37] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Perceptual and Action Systems in Unilateral Visual Neglect , 1987 .
[38] Michael L. Platt,et al. Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.
[39] Linda B. Smith,et al. Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error. , 1999, Psychological review.
[40] D. Kahneman,et al. Functional Imaging of Neural Responses to Expectancy and Experience of Monetary Gains and Losses tasks with monetary payoffs , 2001 .
[41] M. Jeannerod. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. , 1987 .
[42] M. Shaw,et al. Optimal allocation of cognitive resources to spatial locations. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[43] M. Kinsbourne. Mechanisms of Unilateral Neglect , 1987 .
[44] Marlene Behrmann,et al. Mechanisms Underlying Spatial Representation Revealed through Studies of Hemispatial Neglect , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.