Parsing a Cognitive Task: A Characterization of the Mind's Bottleneck

Parsing a mental operation into components, characterizing the parallel or serial nature of this flow, and understanding what each process ultimately contributes to response time are fundamental questions in cognitive neuroscience. Here we show how a simple theoretical model leads to an extended set of predictions concerning the distribution of response time and its alteration by simultaneous performance of another task. The model provides a synthesis of psychological refractory period and random-walk models of response time. It merely assumes that a task consists of three consecutive stages—perception, decision based on noisy integration of evidence, and response—and that the perceptual and motor stages can operate simultaneously with stages of another task, while the central decision process constitutes a bottleneck. We designed a number-comparison task that provided a thorough test of the model by allowing independent variations in number notation, numerical distance, response complexity, and temporal asynchrony relative to an interfering probe task of tone discrimination. The results revealed a parsing of the comparison task in which each variable affects only one stage. Numerical distance affects the integration process, which is the only step that cannot proceed in parallel and has a major contribution to response time variability. The other stages, mapping the numeral to an internal quantity and executing the motor response, can be carried out in parallel with another task. Changing the duration of these processes has no significant effect on the variance.

[1]  W. Smith The Integrative Action of the Nervous System , 1907, Nature.

[2]  Thomas Pègues Traité de l'homme , 2022 .

[3]  C. W. Telford The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .

[4]  R. Thouless Experimental Psychology , 1939, Nature.

[5]  P. J. Foley The foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1959, Canadian journal of psychology.

[6]  R J AUDLEY,et al.  A stochastic model for individual choice behavior. , 1960, Psychological review.

[7]  W. Edwards Optimal strategies for seeking information: Models for statistics, choice reaction times, and human information processing ☆ , 1965 .

[8]  M. C. Smith,et al.  Theories of the psychological refractory period. , 1967, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[10]  R J Audley,et al.  The relation between decision time and relative response frequency in a blue-green discrimination. , 1968, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[11]  Donald Laming,et al.  Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .

[12]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[13]  D. Vickers,et al.  Evidence for an accumulator model of psychophysical discrimination. , 1970, Ergonomics.

[14]  J. Brožek Attention and Performance II. , 1971 .

[15]  A. Greenwald,et al.  On doing two things at once: time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  D. Kahneman Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[17]  A. Greenwald,et al.  On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  S. Link,et al.  A sequential theory of psychological discrimination , 1975 .

[19]  Alfred B. Kristofferson,et al.  Low-variance stimulus-response latencies: Deterministic internal delays? , 1976 .

[20]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[21]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[22]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[23]  M. Posner Chronometric explorations of mind , 1978 .

[24]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[25]  James L. McClelland On the time relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade. , 1979 .

[26]  R. Ratcliff Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  D Laming,et al.  Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[28]  R Schweickert,et al.  Critical-path scheduling of mental processes in a dual task. , 1980, Science.

[29]  C. Eriksen,et al.  A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  R Ratcliff,et al.  Continuous versus discrete information processing modeling accumulation of partial information. , 1988, Psychological review.

[31]  S Dehaene,et al.  The psychophysics of numerical comparison: A reexamination of apparently incompatible data , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  James T. Townsend,et al.  A trichotomy: Interactions of factors prolonging sequential and concurrent mental processes in stochastic discrete mental (PERT) networks , 1989 .

[33]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .

[34]  W. Schwarz Stochastic accumulation of information in discrete time: comparing exact results and Wald approximations , 1990 .

[35]  Carla M. Bosco,et al.  Event-related brain potentials as indices of information extraction and response priming. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[36]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[37]  A. Mecklinger,et al.  Mental comparison: P300 component of the ERP reflects the symbolic distance effect. , 1993, Neuroreport.

[38]  A. Osman,et al.  The locus of dual-task interference: psychological refractory effects on movement-related brain potentials. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[41]  Michael G. H. Coles,et al.  Mental chronometry and the study of human information processing. , 1995 .

[42]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[43]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Visual feature selectivity in frontal eye fields induced by experience in mature macaques , 1996, Nature.

[44]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The Organization of Brain Activations in Number Comparison: Event-Related Potentials and the Additive-Factors Method , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  M N Shadlen,et al.  Motion perception: seeing and deciding. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  K. H. Britten,et al.  A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[48]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  Diederich,et al.  Dynamic Stochastic Models for Decision Making under Time Constraints , 1997, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[50]  D E Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.

[51]  H. Heinze,et al.  On the interaction of numerical and size information in digit comparison: a behavioral and event-related potential study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  H Pashler,et al.  Attentional limitations in dual-task performance. In: Pashler H, editor. Attention. Hove (United Kingdom): Psychology Press/Erlbaum. pp , 1998 .

[53]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Attentional limitations in dual-task performance. , 1998 .

[54]  S. Luck Sources of Dual-Task Interference: Evidence From Human Electrophysiology , 1998 .

[55]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Neural correlates of visual and motor decision processes , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[56]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  Emilio Salinas,et al.  Sensing and deciding in the somatosensory system , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[58]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time. , 1999, Psychological review.

[59]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Decision making: From sensory evidence to a motor command , 2000, Current Biology.

[60]  J. Schall From sensory evidence to a motor command. , 2000, Current biology : CB.

[61]  J. Gold,et al.  Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands , 2000, Nature.

[62]  R W Remington,et al.  A dual-task investigation of automaticity in visual word processing. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[63]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[64]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[65]  S. Sternberg,et al.  Separate modifiability, mental modules, and the use of pure and composite measures to reveal them. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[66]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Attention and performance. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.

[67]  W. Schwarz The ex-Wald distribution as a descriptive model of response times , 2001, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[68]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task , 2001, NeuroImage.

[69]  Jennifer M. Glass,et al.  Virtually Perfect Time Sharing in Dual-Task Performance: Uncorking the Central Cognitive Bottleneck , 2001, Psychological science.

[70]  H. Pashler,et al.  Processing bottlenecks in dual-task performance: Structural limitation or strategic postponement? , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[71]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[72]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging of Visual Recognition , 2001 .

[73]  H. Pashler,et al.  Is dual-task slowing instruction dependent? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[74]  J. Gold,et al.  Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[75]  David E. Kieras,et al.  VIRTUALLY PERFECT TIME SHARING IN DUAL-TASK PERFORMANCE: , 2001 .

[76]  S. Sternberg Separate Modifiability and the Search for Processing Modules , 2002 .

[77]  R. Romo,et al.  Temporal Evolution of a Decision-Making Process in Medial Premotor Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[78]  H. Pashler,et al.  Central bottleneck influences on the processing stages of word production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[79]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[80]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Common Neural Mechanisms for Response Selection and Perceptual Processing , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[81]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Neural Evidence for Representation-Specific Response Selection , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[82]  J. Changeux,et al.  A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[83]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[84]  David A. Medler,et al.  Neural correlates of sensory and decision processes in auditory object identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[85]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of a Perceptual Decision in Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[86]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.

[87]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Provides New Constraints on Theories of the Psychological Refractory Period , 2004, Psychological science.

[88]  John F. Kalaska,et al.  Faculty Opinions recommendation of Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision in ventral premotor cortex. , 2004 .

[89]  Functional neuroimaging of visual cognition: attention and performance XX. N. Kanwisher and J. Duncan (Eds.). Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2004. No. of pages 572. ISBN 0‐19‐852845‐0 , 2005 .