Why the body comes first: effects of experimenter touch on infants' word finding.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Alejandrina Cristia | Amanda Seidl | Ruth Tincoff | R. Tincoff | A. Seidl | Alejandrina Cristia | Christopher Baker | C. Baker
[1] P. Boersma. Praat : doing phonetics by computer (version 4.4.24) , 2006 .
[2] P. Jusczyk,et al. Some Beginnings of Word Comprehension in 6-Month-Olds , 1999 .
[3] LouAnn Gerken,et al. From domain-generality to domain-sensitivity: 4-Month-olds learn an abstract repetition rule in music that 7-month-olds do not , 2009, Cognition.
[4] K. Onishi,et al. Allophonic and Phonemic Contrasts in Infants' Learning of Sound Patterns , 2009, Language Learning and Development.
[5] Marc Sato,et al. Auditory-tactile speech perception in congenitally blind and sighted adults , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[6] Jessica F. Hay,et al. Rhythmic grouping biases constrain infant statistical learning. , 2012, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[7] LouAnn Gerken,et al. Surprise! Infants consider possible bases of generalization for a single input example. , 2014, Developmental science.
[8] A. Majid. Words for parts of the body , 2010 .
[9] E. W. Ames,et al. A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. , 1988 .
[10] Valerie Flynn,et al. Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children's lexical development , 2005, Journal of Child Language.
[11] Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al. Boundary alignment enables 11-month-olds to segment vowel initial words from speech* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[12] Michael D. Tyler,et al. The Edge Factor in Early Word Segmentation: Utterance-Level Prosody Enables Word Form Extraction by 6-Month-Olds , 2014, PloS one.
[13] J. Morgan,et al. Mommy and Me , 2005, Psychological science.
[14] Lyle Campbell,et al. Historical Linguistics: An Introduction , 1991 .
[15] L. Gogate,et al. A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. , 2000, Child development.
[16] Stefano Nolfi,et al. An Experiment on the Evolution of Compositional Semantics and Behaviour Generalisation in Artificial Agents , 2011 .
[17] R N Aslin,et al. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.
[18] Barbara Höhle,et al. Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: evidence from German and French infants. , 2009, Infant behavior & development.
[19] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Acoustic Packaging: Maternal Speech and Action Synchrony , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.
[20] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Stress changes the representational landscape: evidence from word segmentation , 2005, Cognition.
[21] D. Swingley,et al. At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[22] Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al. Language Discrimination by English-Learning 5-Month-Olds: Effects of Rhythm and Familiarity , 2000 .
[23] Matthew J. Hertenstein. Touch: Its Communicative Functions in Infancy , 2002, Human Development.
[24] B. MacWhinney. The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .
[25] B. Gick,et al. Tactile enhancement of auditory and visual speech perception in untrained perceivers. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[26] P. Jusczyk,et al. The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .
[27] D. Muir,et al. Tactile stimulation as a component of social interchange: New interpretations for the still‐face effect , 1990 .
[28] M. Harris,et al. The social context of early sign language development , 1989 .
[29] A. Fernald. Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese" , 1985 .
[30] Paul Boersma,et al. Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .
[31] C. Fowler,et al. Listening with eye and hand: cross-modal contributions to speech perception. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] M. Rowe. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. , 2012, Child development.
[33] R. Cole,et al. Perception and production of fluent speech , 2016 .
[34] C. Spence,et al. The science of interpersonal touch: An overview , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[35] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants' use of synchronized visual information to separate streams of speech. , 2005, Child development.
[36] S. Goldin-Meadow,et al. Differences in Early Gesture Explain SES Disparities in Child Vocabulary Size at School Entry , 2009, Science.
[37] Amanda Seidl,et al. Infant word segmentation revisited: edge alignment facilitates target extraction. , 2006, Developmental science.
[38] Bernd Heine,et al. Cognitive Foundations of Grammar , 1997 .
[39] J. Morgan,et al. Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[40] Daniel Tranel,et al. Searching for the elusive neural substrates of body part terms: A neuropsychological study , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[41] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants′ Detection of the Sound Patterns of Words in Fluent Speech , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.
[42] P. Jusczyk,et al. Six-Month-Olds Comprehend Words That Refer to Parts of the Body. , 2012, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[43] Joost Van de Weijer,et al. Language input for word discovery , 1999 .
[44] Nicolas Guéguen,et al. Another Evaluation of Touch and Helping Behavior , 2003, Psychological reports.
[45] Erik D. Thiessen. Effects of Visual Information on Adults' and Infants' Auditory Statistical Learning , 2010, Cogn. Sci..
[46] D. Pisoni,et al. Infants' Recognition of the Sound Patterns of Their Own Names , 1995, Psychological science.
[47] Elika Bergelson,et al. The acquisition of abstract words by young infants , 2013, Cognition.
[48] Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al. Prosodic temporal alignment of co-speech gestures to speech facilitates referent resolution. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[49] M. Goldstein,et al. Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[50] Erik D. Thiessen,et al. When cues collide: use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. , 2003, Developmental psychology.