Why the body comes first: effects of experimenter touch on infants' word finding.

The lexicon of 6-month-olds is comprised of names and body part words. Unlike names, body part words do not often occur in isolation in the input. This presents a puzzle: How have infants been able to pull out these words from the continuous stream of speech at such a young age? We hypothesize that caregivers' interactions directed at and on the infant's body may be at the root of their early acquisition of body part words. An artificial language segmentation study shows that experimenter-provided synchronous tactile cues help 4-month-olds to find words in continuous speech. A follow-up study suggests that this facilitation cannot be reduced to the highly social situation in which the directed interaction occurs. Taken together, these studies suggest that direct caregiver-infant interaction, exemplified in this study by touch cues, may play a key role in infants' ability to find word boundaries, and suggests that early vocabulary items may consist of words often linked with caregiver touches. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at http://youtu.be/NfCj5ipatyE.

[1]  P. Boersma Praat : doing phonetics by computer (version 4.4.24) , 2006 .

[2]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Some Beginnings of Word Comprehension in 6-Month-Olds , 1999 .

[3]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  From domain-generality to domain-sensitivity: 4-Month-olds learn an abstract repetition rule in music that 7-month-olds do not , 2009, Cognition.

[4]  K. Onishi,et al.  Allophonic and Phonemic Contrasts in Infants' Learning of Sound Patterns , 2009, Language Learning and Development.

[5]  Marc Sato,et al.  Auditory-tactile speech perception in congenitally blind and sighted adults , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Jessica F. Hay,et al.  Rhythmic grouping biases constrain infant statistical learning. , 2012, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[7]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  Surprise! Infants consider possible bases of generalization for a single input example. , 2014, Developmental science.

[8]  A. Majid Words for parts of the body , 2010 .

[9]  E. W. Ames,et al.  A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. , 1988 .

[10]  Valerie Flynn,et al.  Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children's lexical development , 2005, Journal of Child Language.

[11]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Boundary alignment enables 11-month-olds to segment vowel initial words from speech* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[12]  Michael D. Tyler,et al.  The Edge Factor in Early Word Segmentation: Utterance-Level Prosody Enables Word Form Extraction by 6-Month-Olds , 2014, PloS one.

[13]  J. Morgan,et al.  Mommy and Me , 2005, Psychological science.

[14]  Lyle Campbell,et al.  Historical Linguistics: An Introduction , 1991 .

[15]  L. Gogate,et al.  A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. , 2000, Child development.

[16]  Stefano Nolfi,et al.  An Experiment on the Evolution of Compositional Semantics and Behaviour Generalisation in Artificial Agents , 2011 .

[17]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[18]  Barbara Höhle,et al.  Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: evidence from German and French infants. , 2009, Infant behavior & development.

[19]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Acoustic Packaging: Maternal Speech and Action Synchrony , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[20]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Stress changes the representational landscape: evidence from word segmentation , 2005, Cognition.

[21]  D. Swingley,et al.  At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Language Discrimination by English-Learning 5-Month-Olds: Effects of Rhythm and Familiarity , 2000 .

[23]  Matthew J. Hertenstein Touch: Its Communicative Functions in Infancy , 2002, Human Development.

[24]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[25]  B. Gick,et al.  Tactile enhancement of auditory and visual speech perception in untrained perceivers. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .

[27]  D. Muir,et al.  Tactile stimulation as a component of social interchange: New interpretations for the still‐face effect , 1990 .

[28]  M. Harris,et al.  The social context of early sign language development , 1989 .

[29]  A. Fernald Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese" , 1985 .

[30]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .

[31]  C. Fowler,et al.  Listening with eye and hand: cross-modal contributions to speech perception. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  M. Rowe A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. , 2012, Child development.

[33]  R. Cole,et al.  Perception and production of fluent speech , 2016 .

[34]  C. Spence,et al.  The science of interpersonal touch: An overview , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[35]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' use of synchronized visual information to separate streams of speech. , 2005, Child development.

[36]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Differences in Early Gesture Explain SES Disparities in Child Vocabulary Size at School Entry , 2009, Science.

[37]  Amanda Seidl,et al.  Infant word segmentation revisited: edge alignment facilitates target extraction. , 2006, Developmental science.

[38]  Bernd Heine,et al.  Cognitive Foundations of Grammar , 1997 .

[39]  J. Morgan,et al.  Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[40]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  Searching for the elusive neural substrates of body part terms: A neuropsychological study , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[41]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants′ Detection of the Sound Patterns of Words in Fluent Speech , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[42]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Six-Month-Olds Comprehend Words That Refer to Parts of the Body. , 2012, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[43]  Joost Van de Weijer,et al.  Language input for word discovery , 1999 .

[44]  Nicolas Guéguen,et al.  Another Evaluation of Touch and Helping Behavior , 2003, Psychological reports.

[45]  Erik D. Thiessen Effects of Visual Information on Adults' and Infants' Auditory Statistical Learning , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[46]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Infants' Recognition of the Sound Patterns of Their Own Names , 1995, Psychological science.

[47]  Elika Bergelson,et al.  The acquisition of abstract words by young infants , 2013, Cognition.

[48]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Prosodic temporal alignment of co-speech gestures to speech facilitates referent resolution. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  M. Goldstein,et al.  Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[50]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  When cues collide: use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. , 2003, Developmental psychology.