Rod-cone dependence of saccadic eye-movement latency in a foveating task

This study examines the relations between some well known oculomotor functions (saccades) and well known retinal physiology (dark adaptation): it deals with the overall latency versus target luminance functions, with the underlying rod and cone latency-luminance functions, and with the synergistic interaction between these latency functions for mesopic targets. Saccadic latency was measured to small lit targets presented at 10 deg retinal eccentricity in complete darkness. Target luminance and wavelength were varied. Additional measurements were made during dark adaptation or on backgrounds, or at different retinal eccentricities. Luminance matched stimuli and Palmer's (1968) equivalent luminance transformation were also used. Latency is determined by an achromatic luminance mechanism that receives substantial rod inputs above the cone threshold. Latencies for pure rod or pure cone inputs increase rapidly as target luminance decreases. For the rods this latency increase appears to represent the waiting time for the 140 or so photons (lambda = 507 nm) that are required for a saccade. Errors in direction occur at scotopic luminances, or at low photopic luminances when only cones are functioning.

[1]  D. A. Palmer Standard observer for large-field photometry at any level. , 1968, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[2]  D. A. Palmer,et al.  A System of Mesopic Photometry , 1966, Nature.

[3]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Dependence of saccadic eye-movements on stimulus luminance, and an effect of task , 1988, Vision Research.

[4]  P. E. Hallett Rod increment thresholds on steady and flashed backgrounds , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  D. P. Andrews,et al.  Suprathreshold spectral properties of single optic tract fibres in cat, under mesopic adaptation; cone—rod interaction , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  V. Lloyd,et al.  A comparison of critical fusion frequencies for different areas in the fovea and periphery. , 1952, The American journal of psychology.

[7]  B. Stabell,et al.  Variation in density of macular pigmentation and in short-wave cone sensitivity with eccentricity. , 1980, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[8]  H. Vaughan,et al.  Functional Relation between Stimulus Intensity and Photically Evoked Cerebral Responses in Man , 1965, Nature.

[9]  P Gouras,et al.  Rod and cone interaction in dark‐adapted monkey ganglion cells , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  P. Gouras Identification of cone mechanisms in monkey ganglion cells , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  F.J.J. Clarke,et al.  Rapid Light Adaptation of Localised Areas of the Extra-foveal Retina , 1957 .

[12]  R. W. Bowen Latencies for chromatic and achromatic visual mechanisms , 1981, Vision Research.

[13]  J. Ivey,et al.  Ann Arbor, Michigan , 1969 .

[14]  Robert M. Boynton,et al.  Luminance as a Parameter of the Eye-Movement Control System* , 1967 .

[15]  J. Kinney,et al.  Comparison of scotopic, mesopic, and photopic spectral sensitivity curves. , 1958, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[16]  J Gottesman,et al.  Sensory latency and reaction time: dependence on contrast polarity and early linearity in human vision. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[17]  R. T. Brooke,et al.  The variation of critical fusion frequency with brightness of various retinal locations. , 1951, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[18]  C. Enroth-Cugell,et al.  Interactions between the rod and the cone pathways in the cat retina , 1987, Vision Research.

[19]  P. Lennie Recent developments in the physiology of color vision , 1984, Trends in Neurosciences.

[20]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Precise non-contacting measurement of eye movements using the corneal reflex , 1984, Vision Research.

[21]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Quantum efficiency of dark-adapted human vision. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[22]  A. Lit,et al.  Simple time reaction as a function of luminance for various wavelengths , 1971 .

[23]  J. Kinney,et al.  Sensitivity of the eye to spectral radiation at scotopic and mesopic intensity levels. , 1955, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[24]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered by prior saccades , 1976, Vision Research.

[25]  P. King-Smith,et al.  Luminance and opponent-color contributions to visual detection and adaptation and to temporal and spatial integration. , 1976, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[26]  D. Macleod,et al.  Rods Cancel Cones in Flicker , 1972, Nature.

[27]  M. Pirenne,et al.  The minimum flux of energy detectable by the human eye , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[28]  Joel Pokorny,et al.  Wavelength effects on simple reaction time , 1977 .

[29]  N. Bartlett,et al.  Human reaction time during dark adaptation. , 1954, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[30]  A. Lit,et al.  Simple reaction time as a function of luminance for various wavelengths , 1971 .

[31]  P Lennie,et al.  Convergence of rod and cone signals in the cat's retina , 1977, The Journal of physiology.

[32]  F. Marriott The foveal absolute visual threshold for short flashes and small fields , 1963, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  W. D. Wright,et al.  The spectral sensitivity of the fovea and extrafovea in the Purkinje range , 1943, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B - Biological Sciences.

[34]  S. Hecht,et al.  ENERGY, QUANTA, AND VISION , 1942, The Journal of general physiology.

[35]  B. Stabell,et al.  Extrafoveal spectral sensitivity during dark adaptation. , 1980, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[36]  R. L. Valois,et al.  Analysis of response patterns of LGN cells. , 1966, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[37]  L. Chalupa,et al.  Rod-cone interaction in human scotopic vision. I. Temporal analysis. , 1973, Vision research.

[38]  D. A. Palmer,et al.  The definition of a standard observer for mesopic photometry. , 1967, Vision research.

[39]  D. Hood,et al.  Opponent-color cells can influence detection of small, brief lights , 1982, Vision Research.

[40]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Latency functions in human vision. , 1973, Vision research.

[41]  R. Steinman,et al.  Fixation of targets near the absolute foveal threshold. , 1968, Vision research.

[42]  J. Krauskopf,et al.  Reaction time as a measure of the temporal response properties of individua colour mechanisms. , 1973, Vision research.

[43]  B. Stabell,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of the dark-adapted extrafoveal retina at photopic intensities. , 1981, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[44]  A. Goodwin The effect of colour on time delays in the human oculomotor system. , 1973, Vision research.

[45]  H. K. Hartline,et al.  Intensity and duration in the excitation of single photoreceptor units , 1934 .

[46]  Joseph L. Holmes Reaction Time to Photometrically Equal Chromatic Stimuli , 1926 .

[47]  H G Vaughan,et al.  The functional relation of visual evoked response and reaction time to stimulus intensity. , 1966, Vision research.

[48]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Variable contributions of rods and cones to saccadic eye-movement latency in a non-foveating task , 1989, Vision Research.

[49]  L. Temme,et al.  Rod-cone interaction in human scotopic vision—II Cones influence rod increment thresholds , 1977, Vision Research.

[50]  T. Frumkes,et al.  Rod-Cone Interaction in Human Scotopic Vision , 1972, Science.

[51]  J. D. Pollack,et al.  Reaction time to different wavelengths at various luminances , 1968 .

[52]  Vivianne C. Smith,et al.  Reaction times to chromatic stimuli , 1985, Vision Research.