The presence of visual gap affects the duration of stopping process

A milestone on which relies the voluntary control of behavior is the ability to shape our motor output to meet the needs of the context which we are continuously facing. Even though it is solidly established that contextual information influence movement generation few studies have so far explored their effects on inhibitory processes. We compared the inhibitory control of arm movements of ten healthy right-handed volunteers in a countermanding reaching paradigm with and without the presence of a temporal gap between the offset of the central target and the peripheral target appearance. We found that this perceptual gap reduces the reaction times of hand movements and, at the same time, increases the duration of the stop process, the stop signal reaction time. The two effects are not correlated implying that inhibition and execution of reaching movement are two independent processes influenced by a common factor: the disengagement of selective attention from the central target. Therefore our results support the idea of the existence of a link between spatial selective attention and inhibitory processes.

[1]  M. Bravo,et al.  The role of attention in different visual-search tasks , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Reduced response readiness delays stop signal inhibition. , 2002, Acta psychologica.

[3]  Jaap Oosterlaan,et al.  How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder? , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[4]  M. Land,et al.  The Roles of Vision and Eye Movements in the Control of Activities of Daily Living , 1998, Perception.

[5]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Role of focal attention on latencies and trajectories of visually guided manual pointing. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[6]  S. Wise,et al.  The premotor cortex of the monkey , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  R A Abrams,et al.  The Gap effect for spatially oriented responses. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[8]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Visual offsets facilitate saccadic latency: Does predisengagement of visuospatial attention mediate this gap effect? , 1993 .

[9]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  Is interhemispheric transfer of visuomotor information asymmetric? Evidence from a meta-analysis , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  G. Logan,et al.  Converging Evidence for a Fronto-Basal-Ganglia Network for Inhibitory Control of Action and Cognition , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. II. Reversible activation and deactivation. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  S. Ferraina,et al.  Context influences on the preparation and execution of reaching movements , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[14]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Effects of warning signals and fixation point offsets on the latencies of pro- versus antisaccades: implications for an interpretation of the gap effect , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[16]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Anatomical distribution of arm-movement-related neurons in the primate superior colliculus and underlying reticular formation in comparison with visual and saccadic cells , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Estimating the components of the gap effect , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  M. Atienza,et al.  Cortical potentials during gap and non-gap paradigms using manual responses in humans , 1995, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  B Fischer,et al.  The preparation of visually guided saccades. , 1987, Reviews of physiology, biochemistry and pharmacology.

[20]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[21]  B. Fischer,et al.  Gap duration and location of attention focus modulate the occurrence of left/right asymmetries in the saccadic reaction times of human subjects , 1995, Vision Research.

[22]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Inhibitory Control is Slowed in Patients with Right Superior Medial Frontal Damage , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Switching from automatic to controlled action by monkey medial frontal cortex , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  What neural pathways mediate express saccades? , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[25]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Express attentional shifts , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  Geert J. M. van Boxtel,et al.  Stimulation of the Subthalamic Region Facilitates the Selection and Inhibition of Motor Responses in Parkinson's Disease , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  D. Munoz,et al.  Inhibitory control of eye movements during oculomotor countermanding in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[29]  Okihide Hikosaka,et al.  A Code for Behavioral Inhibition on the Basis of Color, But Not Motion, in Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex of Macaque Monkey , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  A. Riehle,et al.  The predictive value for performance speed of preparatory changes in neuronal activity of the monkey motor and premotor cortex , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  A. Nambu,et al.  Functional significance of the cortico–subthalamo–pallidal ‘hyperdirect’ pathway , 2002, Neuroscience Research.

[32]  D. P. Hanes,et al.  Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  J. Mink The Basal Ganglia and involuntary movements: impaired inhibition of competing motor patterns. , 2003, Archives of neurology.

[34]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Attentional selection during preparation of prehension movements , 2003 .

[35]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Premotor cortical ablations in monkeys: contralateral changes in visually guided reaching behavior. , 1977, Science.

[36]  K. Sasaki,et al.  Suppression of visually initiated hand movement by stimulation of the prefrontal cortex in the monkey , 1989, Brain Research.

[37]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Correlation of primate superior colliculus and reticular formation discharge with proximal limb muscle activity. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Manual response preparation and saccade programming are linked to attention shifts: ERP evidence for covert attentional orienting and spatially specific modulations of visual processing , 2006, Brain Research.

[39]  B. Breitmeyer,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention revealed by saccadic eye movements , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Adam P. Morris,et al.  Executive Brake Failure following Deactivation of Human Frontal Lobe , 2006 .

[41]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Perseverative interference in monkeys following selective lesions of the inferior prefrontal convexity , 1970, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  C. Kennard,et al.  The role of the pre-supplementary motor area in the control of action , 2007, NeuroImage.

[43]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Immediate post-saccadic information mediates space constancy , 1998, Vision Research.

[44]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[45]  Nicholas L. Port,et al.  Motor Cortical Activity during Interception of Moving Targets. , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Aging and inhibition: beyond a unitary view of inhibitory processing in attention. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[47]  W. C. Hall,et al.  The Superior Colliculus : New Approaches for Studying Sensorimotor Integration , 2003 .

[48]  H. Bekkering,et al.  The gap effect for eye and hand movements , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  J. Pratt,et al.  The role of the gap effect in the orienting of attention: Evidence for express attentional shifts , 2000 .

[51]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas in the frontal lobe of the primate , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.

[52]  M. W. Molen,et al.  A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychology.

[53]  W. Byblow,et al.  Intracortical inhibition during volitional inhibition of prepared action. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[55]  Alexa Riehle,et al.  Are extent and force independent movement parameters? Preparation- and movement-related neuronal activity in the monkey cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  Robert D. Rafal,et al.  Strategic control over saccadic eye movements: Studies of the fixation offset effect , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[57]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Deciding not to GO: neuronal correlates of response selection in a GO/NOGO task in primate premotor and parietal cortex. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[58]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  CONCURRENT, DISTRIBUTED CONTROL OF SACCADE INITIATION IN THE FRONTAL EYE FIELD AND SUPERIOR COLLICULUS , 2003 .

[59]  I. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .

[60]  G. B. Wetherill,et al.  Sequential Estimation of Quantal Response Curves , 1963 .

[61]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[62]  Okihide Hikosaka,et al.  Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey subthalamic nucleus. , 1992 .

[63]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  A study of adaptive behavior: effects of age and irrelevant information on the ability to inhibit one's actions , 1999 .

[64]  T. Shallice,et al.  A Multidisciplinary Approach to Anterior Attentional Functions a , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[65]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Stopping eye and hand movements: Are the processes independent? , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.

[66]  G. Barrie Wetherill,et al.  Sequential methods in statistics , 1967 .

[67]  B. Fischer,et al.  Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[68]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of cell discharge. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[69]  J Miller,et al.  Effects of preliminary perceptual output on neuronal activity of the primary motor cortex. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[70]  A. Kingstone,et al.  Fixation offset and stop signal intensity effects on saccadic countermanding: a crossmodal investigation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[71]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Spatial attention-determined modifications in saccade trajectories. , 1995, Neuroreport.

[72]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[73]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[74]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[75]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Confirmation of an Inhibitory Control Deficit in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2000, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[76]  D P Munoz,et al.  Saccadic reaction time in the monkey: advanced preparation of oculomotor programs is primarily responsible for express saccade occurrence. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[77]  C. Lajonchere,et al.  Attentional modulation of the gap effect , 2006, Vision Research.

[78]  T. Shallice,et al.  Effects of focal frontal lesions on response inhibition. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[79]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  The duration of response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm varies with response force. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[80]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Saccadic countermanding: a comparison of central and peripheral stop signals , 2001, Vision Research.

[81]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Don’t look! don’t touch! inhibitory control of eye and hand movements , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[82]  Pierpaolo Pani,et al.  Inhibitory control of reaching movements in humans , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[83]  J. Requin,et al.  Changes in neuronal activity of the monkey precentral cortex during preparation for movement. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[84]  G. Logan,et al.  Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control , 1997 .

[85]  Maurits W. van der Molen,et al.  The duration of response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm varies with response force , 2003 .

[86]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[87]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  A possible role of the superior colliculus in eye-hand coordination. , 2001, Progress in brain research.

[88]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Fixation offset facilitates saccades and manual reaching for single but not multiple target displays , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[89]  Mary M Hayhoe,et al.  Visual memory and motor planning in a natural task. , 2003, Journal of vision.