Neural encoding and production of functional morphemes in the posterior temporal lobe

Morphemes are the smallest meaning-carrying units in human language, and are among the most basic building blocks through which humans express specific ideas and concepts. By using time-resolved cortical stimulations, neural recordings, and focal lesion evaluations, we show that inhibition of a small cortical area within the left dominant posterior–superior temporal lobe selectively impairs the ability to produce appropriate functional morphemes but does not distinctly affect semantic and lexical retrieval, comprehension, or articulation. Additionally, neural recordings within this area reveal the localized encoding of morphological properties and their planned production prior to speech onset. Finally, small lesions localized to the gray matter in this area result in a selective functional morpheme-production deficit. Collectively, these findings reveal a detailed division of linguistic labor within the posterior–superior temporal lobe and suggest that functional morpheme processing constitutes an operationally discrete step in the series of computations essential to language production.Functional morphemes allow us to express details about objects, events, and their relationships. Here, authors show that inhibiting a small cortical area within left posterior superior temporal lobe selectively impairs the ability to produce functional morphemes but does not impair other linguistic abilities.

[1]  Sylvia Vitello,et al.  Localising semantic and syntactic processing in spoken and written language comprehension: An Activation Likelihood Estimation meta-analysis , 2015, Brain and Language.

[2]  R. Levy Expectation-based syntactic comprehension , 2008, Cognition.

[3]  R. Knight,et al.  Redefining the role of Broca’s area in speech , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Mirela V. Simon Intraoperative Neurophysiology: A Comprehensive Guide to Monitoring and Mapping , 2009 .

[5]  Pascale Tremblay,et al.  Broca and Wernicke are dead, or moving past the classic model of language neurobiology , 2016, Brain and Language.

[6]  D. Poeppel,et al.  Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language , 2004, Cognition.

[7]  N. Barbaro,et al.  Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Word Processing in the Human Brain , 2007, Front. Neurosci..

[8]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Evidence of syntactic deficits in a fluent aphasic , 1986, Brain and Language.

[9]  V. Ferreira,et al.  The Oxford Handbook of Language Production , 2014 .

[10]  N. Geschwind The Organization of Language and the Brain: Language disorders after brain damage help in elucidating the neural basis of verbal behavior , 1970 .

[11]  Kenneth H. Rubin,et al.  The development and treatment of childhood aggression. , 1991 .

[12]  A. Friederici,et al.  Broca's area in the human brain is involved in the selection of grammatical gender for language production: evidence from event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[13]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Lexical and syntactic representations in the brain: An fMRI investigation with multi-voxel pattern analyses , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Alexa Bautista,et al.  Neural responses to grammatically and lexically degraded speech , 2016, Language, cognition and neuroscience.

[15]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Young children's sensitivity to probabilistic phonotactics in the developing lexicon. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[16]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Supplementary Information for Natural Speech Reveals the Semantic Maps That Tile Human Cerebral Cortex , 2022 .

[17]  Katherine S White,et al.  Toddlers use speech disfluencies to predict speakers' referential intentions. , 2011, Developmental science.

[18]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[19]  Shaun R. Patel,et al.  Human Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Neurons Mediate Ongoing Behavioral Adaptation , 2012, Nature.

[20]  Darin D Dougherty,et al.  Studying task-related activity of individual neurons in the human brain , 2013, Nature Protocols.

[21]  N. Geschwind The organization of language and the brain. , 1970, Science.

[22]  Keith Johnson,et al.  Phonetic Feature Encoding in Human Superior Temporal Gyrus , 2014, Science.

[23]  Volkmar Glauche,et al.  Ventral and dorsal pathways for language , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[24]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[25]  Yasmeen Faroqi-Shah,et al.  Are regular and irregular verbs dissociated in non-fluent aphasia? A meta-analysis , 2007, Brain Research Bulletin.

[26]  Frank H. Guenther,et al.  An fMRI investigation of syllable sequence production , 2006, NeuroImage.

[27]  Evelina Fedorenko,et al.  Syntactic processing is distributed across the language system , 2016, NeuroImage.

[28]  Gina R Kuperberg,et al.  What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? , 2016, Language, cognition and neuroscience.

[29]  D. Mirman,et al.  Neural Organization of Spoken Language Revealed by Lesion-Symptom Mapping , 2015, Nature Communications.

[30]  P Hagoort,et al.  A neural correlate of syntactic encoding during speech production , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  Shaun R. Patel,et al.  Encoding of rules by neurons in the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  H. H. Clark Speech errors as linguistic evidence. , 1975 .

[33]  Israel Nelken,et al.  Responses of auditory-cortex neurons to structural features of natural sounds , 1999, Nature.

[34]  N. Mesgarani,et al.  Selective cortical representation of attended speaker in multi-talker speech perception , 2012, Nature.

[35]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  Tara McAllister Byun,et al.  Functional Segregation of Cortical Regions Underlying Speech Timing and Articulation , 2016, Neuron.

[37]  Bruce L. Miller,et al.  Syntactic Processing Depends on Dorsal Language Tracts , 2011, Neuron.

[38]  Christopher M. Bishop,et al.  Neural networks for pattern recognition , 1995 .

[39]  S. Edwards,et al.  Profiling fluent aphasic spontaneous speech: a comparison of two methodologies. , 1995, European journal of disorders of communication : the journal of the College of Speech and Language Therapists, London.

[40]  M. Mesulam,et al.  The Wernicke conundrum and the anatomy of language comprehension in primary progressive aphasia. , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  Kristofer E. Bouchard,et al.  Functional Organization of Human Sensorimotor Cortex for Speech Articulation , 2013, Nature.

[42]  John Del Gaizo,et al.  Activity associated with speech articulation measured through direct cortical recordings , 2017, Brain and Language.

[43]  Evelina Fedorenko,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Language and Thought Are Not the Same Thing: Evidence from Neuroimaging and Neurological Patients , 2022 .

[44]  Mirela V. Simon,et al.  The Variability of Stimulus Thresholds in Electrophysiologic Cortical Language Mapping , 2011, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[45]  Evelina Fedorenko,et al.  Functional Characterization of the Human Speech Articulation Network , 2018, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  Susan Edwards,et al.  Word order and finiteness in Dutch and English Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia , 2004, Brain and Language.

[47]  W. Levelt,et al.  The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components , 2004, Cognition.

[48]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia , 1976, Brain and Language.

[49]  Philip A. Cook,et al.  Heteromodal conceptual processing in the angular gyrus , 2013, NeuroImage.

[50]  Jayaram Chandrashekar,et al.  Sequential Processing of Lexical, Grammatical, and Phonological Information Within Broca's Area , 2009 .

[51]  Morris Halle,et al.  The rules of language , 1980, IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication.

[52]  Larry Wasserman,et al.  All of Statistics: A Concise Course in Statistical Inference , 2004 .

[53]  Sarah M. E. Gierhan,et al.  Shared Language , 2011, Psychological science.

[54]  Hossein Karimi,et al.  Good-enough linguistic representations and online cognitive equilibrium in language processing , 2016, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[55]  B. Rapp,et al.  Lexical and post-lexical phonological representations in spoken production , 2007, Cognition.

[56]  G. Ojemann,et al.  Cortical language localization in left, dominant hemisphere. An electrical stimulation mapping investigation in 117 patients. , 1989, Journal of neurosurgery.

[57]  Mario Aguilar,et al.  Predicting Neural Activity Patterns Associated with Sentences Using a Neurobiologically Motivated Model of Semantic Representation , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[58]  P. Fox,et al.  Resting-state functional reorganization in Parkinson's disease: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis , 2017, Cortex.

[59]  Peter Indefrey,et al.  The Spatial and Temporal Signatures of Word Production Components: A Critical Update , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[60]  H. Duffau,et al.  Intraoperative electrical stimulation in awake craniotomy: methodological aspects of current practice. , 2010, Neurosurgical focus.

[61]  L. Cohen,et al.  The role of the supplementary motor area (SMA) in word production , 2006, Brain Research.

[62]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  A dorsal-pathway account of aphasic language production: The WEAVER++/ARC model , 2014, Cortex.

[63]  E. F. Chang,et al.  Sub-centimeter language organization in the human temporal lobe , 2011, Brain and Language.

[64]  Christopher Rorden,et al.  Revealing the dual streams of speech processing , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.