Transitive choices by a simple, fully connected, backpropagation neural network: implications for the comparative study of transitive inference.

Abstract. In search of the minimal requirements for transitive reasoning, a simple neural network was trained and tested on the non-verbal version of the conventional "five-term-series task" – a paradigm used with human adults, children and a variety of non-human species. The transitive performance of the network was analogous in several aspects to that reported for children and animals. The three effects usually associated with transitive choices i.e. "symbolic distance", "lexical marking" and "end-anchor", were also clearly shown by the neural network. In a second experiment, where the training conditions were manipulated, the network failed to match the behavioural pattern reported for human adults in the test following an ordered presentation of the premises. However, it mimicked young children's performance when tested with a novel comparison term. Although we do not intend to suggest a new model of transitive inference, we conclude, in line with other authors, that a simple error-correcting rule can generate transitive behaviour similar to the choice pattern of children and animals in the binary form of the five-term-series task without requiring high-order logical or paralogical abilities. The analysis of the training history and of the final internal structure of the network reveals the associative strategy employed. However, our results indicate that the scope of the associative strategy used by the network might be limited. The extent to which the conventional five-term-series task, in absence of appropriate manipulations of training and testing conditions, is suitable to detect cognitive differences across species is also discussed on the basis of our results.

[1]  C. Wynne Reinforcement accounts for transitive inference performance , 1995 .

[2]  M R D'Amato,et al.  Representation of serial order in monkeys (Cebus apella). , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[3]  Juan D Delius,et al.  Transitive responding in animals and humans: Exaptation rather than adaptation? , 1998, Behavioural Processes.

[4]  T. Trabasso,et al.  Transitive Inferences and Memory in Young Children , 1971, Nature.

[5]  K. O’regan,et al.  What Children Do in Spite of Adults' Hypotheses , 1973, Nature.

[6]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Individual Choice Behavior , 1959 .

[7]  William A. Roberts,et al.  Transitive Inference in Rats: A Test of the Spatial Coding Hypothesis , 1994 .

[8]  K. Kallio,et al.  Developmental change on a five-term transitive inference. , 1982, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[9]  B. McGonigle,et al.  A model of Transitive Choice , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[10]  C De Lillio,et al.  The serial organisation of behaviour by non-human primates; an evaluation of experimental paradigms. , 1996, Behavioural brain research.

[11]  H. Davis Transitive inference in rats (Rattus norvegicus). , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.

[12]  M. Siemann,et al.  Influences of task concreteness upon transitive responding in humans , 1996, Psychological research.

[13]  D. Gillan Reasoning in the chimpanzee: II. Transitive inference. , 1981 .

[14]  T. Zentall,et al.  Determinants of value transfer and contrast in simultaneous discriminations by pigeons , 2000 .

[15]  M. Bitterman,et al.  A conventional conditioning analysis of "transitive inference" in pigeons. , 1992 .

[16]  Janice N. Steirn,et al.  Transitive inference in pigeons: Simplified procedures and a test of value transfer theory , 1995 .

[17]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[18]  Training requirements and retention characteristics of serial list organization by macaque monkeys , 1999, Animal Cognition.

[19]  Carlo De Lillo,et al.  Review article , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Herbert S. Terrace,et al.  The Phylogeny and Ontogeny of Serial Memory: List Learning by Pigeons and Monkeys , 1993 .

[21]  J. Piaget Judgement and Reasoning in the Child , 1962 .

[22]  J. Staddon,et al.  Pigeons' inferences are transitive and the outcome of elementary conditioning principles: A response. , 1992 .

[23]  T. Trabasso,et al.  Comparatives, logical structures, and encoding in a transitive inference task. , 1974 .

[24]  Brendan McGonigle,et al.  Monkeys are Rational! , 1992 .

[25]  J. Staddon,et al.  Transitive inference formation in pigeons. , 1991 .

[26]  Herbert S. Terrace,et al.  Memory and Representation of Serial Order by Children, Monkeys, and Pigeons , 1994 .

[27]  Martina Siemann Juan D. Delius Algebraic Learning and Neural Network Models for Transitive and Non-transitive Responding , 1998 .

[28]  B. McGonigle,et al.  Are monkeys logical? , 1977, Nature.

[29]  J J Higa,et al.  "Transitive inference" in multiple conditional discriminations. , 1993, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[30]  C. D. L. Wynne,et al.  Pigeon transitive inference: Tests of simple accounts of a complex performance , 1997, Behavioural Processes.

[31]  Janice N. Steirn,et al.  Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer , 1997 .

[32]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  The hippocampus and memory for orderly stimulus relations. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  A A Smirnova,et al.  Transitive inference in hooded crows: preliminary data. , 2000, Doklady biological sciences : proceedings of the Academy of Sciences of the USSR, Biological sciences sections.

[34]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning representations by back-propagating errors , 1986, Nature.

[35]  L. Breslow Reevaluation of the Literature on the Development of Transitive Inferences. , 1981 .