Romance clitic pronouns appear to the left of the verb in I and to the right of the verb in C. This alternation correlates with: (a) allomorphy, specifically lvs. zero; (b) stress shifts; and (c) reordering of the clitic string. The alternations in (a)–(c) are also observed between non-negative and negative contexts. The key points of our analysis are: (i) the lsegment is associated with definite content; (ii) interpretively, pronouns scope out of modal/non-veridical operators; (iii) syntactically, the exponent for modality/nonveridicality may have the pronoun in its domain; (iv) externalization of the lsegment is found when semantic scope (ii) and syntactic configuration (iii) are mismatched. Therefore allomorphies (including stress), far from being morphophonological quirks, contribute to the externalization of syntactico-semantic notions of non-veridicality. In dealing with clitic (re)ordering we propose a model based on the dissociation between Merge and linear order. Phrasal constituents are ordered to the right of the verb in Romance; clitics mirror them in that they are ordered to the left, while keeping the Merge relations constant. 1. BASIC EVIDENCE AND CURRENT ACCOUNTS In many Romance varieties, the preverbal and postverbal positions of clitics correlate with a number of morphophonological and order alternations. There are three main such alternations, namely: (a) segmental allomorphies; (b) stress shifts; and (c) internal reordering of the clitic string – which are illustrated in sections 1.1–1.3 below. Some segmental alternations affecting Romance clitics are phonologically conditioned. For instance, in the Corsican variety of Zonza, vocalic proclitics, as in (1a), precede only consonantal onsets; vocalic onsets are preceded by an l form, as in (1b). This condition is phonological, involving presumably the resyllabification of the l segment with the following nucleus. 1 The research reported in this article was partially supported by the PRIN 2012 project grant on Long Distance Dependencies. We thank Adam Ledgeway and Diego Pescarini for detailed comments on the first version of this work. We use a broad transcription for the dialectal data. Word stress is indicated selectively, namely when it occurs on the antepenultimate syllable or on the last syllable of polysyllabic words. For the sake of clarity we indicate stress also in instances of enclisis where stress alternations are involved. Hyphens are introduced in IPA transcriptions and in the glosses to facilitate processing of the data; following the French orthographic conventions, they are used to separate the verb and its enclitics. Transactions of the Philological Society Volume 115:1 (2017) 98–136 doi: 10.1111/1467-968X.12093 © The authors 2016. Transactions of the Philological Society © The Philological Society 2016. Published by John Wiley & Sons, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. (1) a. u/a/i 'cammani him/her/them they.call ‘They call him/her/them’ b. l ani camat-u/a/i him/her/them they.have called-msg/fsg/pl ‘They called him/her/them’ Zonza In this article, however, we argue for the conclusion that enclisis/proclisis segmental alternations, as well as stress alternations and reordering, are determined by syntacticosemantic conditions. Hauser et al. (2002) and Berwick & Chomsky (2011) conceive the human Faculty of Language (FL) as consisting of an inner core (the Narrow FL or FLN) which includes the recursive operations of syntax (the computational component) and of a number of components recruited by the FLN (the so-called Broad FL or FLB). The FLB includes both Conceptual-Intentional (CI) components (shared with general cognition, such as inferential and conceptual systems) and Sensory-Motor (SM) components, in practice articulatory and perceptual systems – which preside to the ‘externalization’ of the mental representations created by syntax and CI systems. In terms of this model, our thesis is that segmental and stress enclisis/proclisis alternations are not internal quirks of the SM system, but contribute to the externalization of syntactic computation and of the CI content entering into it. A crucial role in these alternations is played by the lexicon. Thus the lalternation in (1) must involve the existence of two lexical entries, namely l and the u/a/i series (see also section 2.1). The reason is that there is no general process of linsertion or ldeletion in Romance which could independently motivate it. This lexical fact is in itself arbitrary; for instance, in standard Italian, all 3rd person clitics involve l-. In other words, the child who learns Corsican or Italian must learn the list of clitics (and cannot infer it from Universal Grammar). This does not prevent us from looking for general SM (phonological) or syntactico-semantic principles that govern the distribution of lexical forms and of their allomorphs and that in innatist models, favour the acquisition of idiosyncrasies by the child who learns the language. We follow Distributed Morphology (DM; Halle and Marantz 1993), in assuming that the lexicon lists elementary units corresponding to morphemes and that the same Merge operation responsible for syntactic constituent structure also yields word formation from elementary morphemes. On the other hand, the conclusions on enclisis/proclisis alternations anticipated above can be read as supporting the idea that there is no morphological buffer component between the syntactico-semantic computational core and its SM externalization, of the type provided by such constructs as Morphological Structure and Late Insertion in DM. In sections 1.1–1.4 we will use only a very elementary syntactic model. The fundamental sentential tree is defined in Chomsky’s (1995) terms by the category V(erb) – or technically by the complex v-V – dominated by the category I(nflection) and by the category C(complementizer). Kayne (1991) proposes that proclisis on the finite verb in Romance, e.g. in Italian (2a), depends on the I position of the verb; enclisis on the infinitive, as in (2b), depends on the verb moving to C, while the clitic maintains the same position as in (2a). This is illustrated in structures (3a–b). Using enclisis/proclisis alternations as a diagnostics for 2 An anonymous reviewer mentions historical change as the sole source of explanation for the shape of the lexicon. This seems to us an oversimplification, insofar as the lexicon must present an internal organization suitable for being learnt by the children of each new generation. 3 The adoption of morpheme-based morphology automatically excludes paradigm-based approaches. This theoretical choice characterizes a larger spectrum of models than just generative ones. MANZINI & SAVOIA – ENCLISIS/PROCLISIS ALTERNATIONS 99
[1]
J. Melander.
L'origine de l'italien me ne, me 1o, te 1a, etc.
,
1929
.
[2]
A. Lombard.
Le Groupement des pronoms personnels régimes atones en italien
,
1934
.
[3]
Richard S. Kayne.
Connectedness and binary branching
,
1984
.
[4]
Richard S. Kayne.
Romance Clitics, Verb Movement and PRO
,
1991
.
[5]
Christopher Laenzlinger,et al.
Enclitic clustering: the case of french positive imperatives
,
1994
.
[6]
Richard S. Kayne.
The Antisymmetry of Syntax
,
1994
.
[7]
A. Carnie,et al.
Papers on phonology and morphology
,
1994
.
[8]
M. Rivero.
Clause structure and V-movement in the languages of the Balkans
,
1994
.
[9]
Alec Marantz,et al.
No escape from syntax: Don't try morphological analysis in the privacy of your own lexicon
,
1997
.
[10]
L. Rizzi.
The Fine Structure of the Left Periphery
,
1997
.
[11]
Arhonto Terzi.
Clitic Combinations, Their Hosts and Their Ordering
,
1999
.
[12]
Lori Repetti,et al.
Phonological theory and the dialects of Italy
,
2000
.
[13]
M. Loporcaro.
Stress Stability Under Cliticization and the Prosodic Status of Romance Clitics
,
2000
.
[14]
A. Zamboni.
Alle origini dell'italiano : dinamiche e tipologie della transizione dal latino
,
2000
.
[15]
M. Brody,et al.
Mirror Theory: Syntactic Representation in Perfect Syntax
,
2000,
Linguistic Inquiry.
[16]
Noam Chomsky,et al.
The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?
,
2002
.
[17]
Christina Tortora,et al.
Romance Enclisis, Prepositions, and Aspect
,
2002
.
[18]
Heidi Harley,et al.
Possession and the double object construction
,
2002
.
[19]
Leonardo M. Savoia,et al.
Uninterpretable features are incompatible in morphology with other minimalist postulates
,
2004
.
[20]
A. Cardinaletti,et al.
Clitics in Northern Italian Dialects: Phonology, Syntax and Microvariation
,
2004
.
[21]
M. Manzini,et al.
I dialetti italiani e romanci : morfosintassi generativa
,
2005
.
[22]
Noam Chomsky.
Three Factors in Language Design
,
2005,
Linguistic Inquiry.
[23]
James Harris,et al.
Unexpected Plural Inflections in Spanish: Reduplication and Metathesis
,
2005,
Linguistic Inquiry.
[24]
Adam Ledgeway,et al.
Verb Movement, Adverbs and Clitic Positions in Romance
,
2005
.
[25]
Valentina Bianchi,et al.
On the syntax of personal arguments
,
2006
.
[26]
Leonardo M. Savoia,et al.
A Unification of Morphology and Syntax: Investigations into Romance and Albanian Dialects
,
2006
.
[27]
Anna Cardinaletti,et al.
The Phonology and Syntax of Preverbal and Postverbal Subject Clitics in Northern Italian Dialects
,
2008,
Linguistic Inquiry.
[28]
A. Cardinaletti,et al.
The Phonology and Syntax of Subject Clitics in Interrogative Sentences
,
2008
.
[29]
Raffaella Zanuttini,et al.
Encoding the addressee in the syntax: evidence from English imperative subjects
,
2008
.
[30]
Peter Svenonius,et al.
Mapping a parochial lexicon onto a universal semantics
,
2008
.
[31]
Judy B. Bernstein.
Reformulating the Determiner Phrase Analysis
,
2008,
Language and Linguistics Compass.
[32]
A. Kratzer.
Making a Pronoun: Fake Indexicals as Windows into the Properties of Pronouns
,
2009,
Linguistic Inquiry.
[33]
Adam Ledgeway.
Grammatica diacronica del napoletano
,
2009
.
[34]
Richard S. Kayne.
Toward a syntactic reinterpretation of Harris & Halle (2005)
,
2010
.
[35]
Alessandra Giorgi,et al.
About the Speaker: Towards a Syntax of Indexicality
,
2010
.
[36]
Leonardo M. Savoia,et al.
Mesoclisis in the imperative: Phonology, morphology or syntax?
,
2011
.
[37]
D. Delfitto,et al.
Person Features and Pronominal Anaphora
,
2011,
Linguistic Inquiry.
[38]
Adam Ledgeway.
From Latin to Romance: Morphosyntactic Typology and Change
,
2012
.
[39]
Heidi Harley,et al.
External arguments and the Mirror Principle: On the distinctness of Voice and v
,
2013
.
[40]
Noam Chomsky,et al.
Problems of projection
,
2013
.
[41]
S. Wurmbrand.
Tense and Aspect in English Infinitives
,
2014,
Linguistic Inquiry.
[42]
Leonardo M. Savoia,et al.
From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems
,
2014
.
[43]
M. R. Manzini,et al.
Grammatical categories: Strong and weak pronouns in Romance
,
2014
.
[44]
F. Ordóñez,et al.
On the morphological restrictions of hosting clitics in Italian and Sardinian dialects
,
2014
.
[45]
Anna Roussou,et al.
Middle-passive voice in Albanian and Greek1
,
2015,
Journal of Linguistics.
[46]
Heather Bliss,et al.
Temporal restrictions on personal pronouns: The composition of Blackfoot proclitics
,
2015
.
[47]
Tim Stowell,et al.
THE TENSE OF INFINITIVES
,
2016
.
[48]
M. R. Manzini,et al.
Goal and DOM datives
,
2016
.
[49]
H. Müller.
Typological correlations in nominal determination in Romance
,
2017
.
[50]
M. Loporcaro.
I dialetti italiani
,
2017
.
[51]
Doris Penka,et al.
1 Negative and Positive Polarity Items
,
2017
.