A first step in form-based category abstraction by 12-month-old infants.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] L. Frank. The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .
[2] Kirk H. Smith,et al. Learning Co-occurrence restrictions: Rule induction or rote learning? , 1969 .
[3] D. Homa. Prototype abstraction and classification of new instances as a function of number of instances defining the prototype , 1973 .
[4] H. Toutenburg,et al. Lehmann, E. L., Nonparametrics: Statistical Methods Based on Ranks, San Francisco. Holden‐Day, Inc., 1975. 480 S., $ 22.95 . , 1977 .
[5] Elissa L. Newport,et al. The role of constituent structure in the induction of an artificial language , 1981 .
[6] D. Slobin,et al. Children use canonical sentence schemas: A crosslinguistic study of word order and inflections , 1982, Cognition.
[7] Eric Wanner,et al. Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .
[8] Eric Wanner,et al. A technology anthology. Recent writings and remarks on the state of the state-of-the-art. , 1982, Health systems review.
[9] C. L. Baker,et al. The Logical problem of language acquisition , 1984 .
[10] Steven Pinker,et al. Language learnability and language development , 1985 .
[11] Elissa L Newport,et al. Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[12] L. Gleitman,et al. Language and Experience: Evidence from the Blind Child , 1988 .
[13] Virginia Valian,et al. Anchor points in language learning: The role of marker frequency ☆ , 1988 .
[14] P. Jusczyk,et al. A precursor of language acquisition in young infants , 1988, Cognition.
[15] Dorrit Billman,et al. Systems of correlations in rule and category learning: Use of structured input in learning syntactic categories , 1989 .
[16] Allard Jongman,et al. Phonological and form class relations in the lexicon , 1990 .
[17] Barbara Landau,et al. Function Morphemes in Young Children's Speech Perception and Production , 1990 .
[18] L. Gleitman. The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings , 2020, Sentence First, Arguments Afterward.
[19] M. H. Kelly,et al. Phonological information for grammatical category assignments , 1991 .
[20] Eleanor J. Gibson,et al. An Odyssey in Learning and Perception , 1991 .
[21] M. H. Kelly,et al. Using sound to solve syntactic problems: the role of phonology in grammatical category assignments. , 1992, Psychological review.
[22] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. , 1993, Child development.
[23] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. , 1994 .
[24] P. Jusczyk,et al. The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .
[25] R N Aslin,et al. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.
[26] Harlene Hayne,et al. Developmental changes in the specificity of memory over the second year of life , 1997 .
[27] P. Jusczyk. The discovery of spoken language , 1997 .
[28] Paul D. Allopenna,et al. Phonological and acoustic bases for earliest grammatical category assignment: a cross-linguistic perspective , 1998, Journal of Child Language.
[29] E. Newport,et al. Computation of Conditional Probability Statistics by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1998 .
[30] J. Mehler,et al. Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] P. Jusczyk,et al. Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: evidence for limitations in processing space , 1998, Cognition.
[32] Janet L. McDonald,et al. Properties of Phonological Markers That Affect the Acquisition of Gender-Like Subclasses☆☆☆★ , 1998 .
[33] L. Gerken,et al. An electrophysiological study of infants' sensitivity to the sound patterns of English speech. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[34] Nick Chater,et al. Distributional Information: A Powerful Cue for Acquiring Syntactic Categories , 1998, Cogn. Sci..
[35] Carolyn Rovee-Collier,et al. The Development of Infant Memory , 1999 .
[36] Peter M. Vishton,et al. Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.
[37] J. Werker,et al. Newborn infants’ sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words , 1999, Cognition.
[38] R. Gómez,et al. Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge , 1999, Cognition.
[39] R. Schvaneveldt,et al. The basis of transfer in artificial grammar learning , 2000, Memory & cognition.
[40] Harlene Hayne,et al. Memory retrieval by 18--30-month-olds: age-related changes in representational flexibility. , 2000 .
[41] J. Mandler,et al. Advancing Downward to the Basic Level , 2000 .
[42] H. Hayne,et al. The Ontogeny of Long‐Term Retention During the Second Year of Life , 2000 .
[43] R. Gómez,et al. Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[44] H. Hayne,et al. Memory retrieval by 18--30-month-olds: age-related changes in representational flexibility. , 2000, Developmental psychology.
[45] LouAnn Gerken,et al. Signal to syntax : bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition , 1996 .
[46] Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al. Word Segmentation by 8-Month-Olds: When Speech Cues Count More Than Statistics , 2001 .
[47] Scott P. Johnson,et al. Visual statistical learning in infancy: evidence for a domain general learning mechanism , 2002, Cognition.
[48] Letitia R. Naigles,et al. Form is easy, meaning is hard: resolving a paradox in early child language , 2002, Cognition.
[49] Toben H. Mintz. Category induction from distributional cues in an artificial language , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[50] R. Gómez. Variability and Detection of Invariant Structure , 2002, Psychological science.
[51] E. Newport,et al. The Development of Language from Non-native Linguistic Input , 2002 .
[52] LouAnn Gerken,et al. Early sensitivity to linguistic form , 2002 .
[53] Erik D. Thiessen,et al. When cues collide: use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. , 2003, Developmental psychology.
[54] S. Waxman. Links between object categorization and naming: Origins and emergence in human infants , 2003 .
[55] J. Saffran,et al. From Syllables to Syntax: Multilevel Statistical Learning by 12-Month-Old Infants , 2003 .
[56] Barbara Höhle,et al. German-learning infants' ability to detect unstressed closed class elements in continuous speech , 2003 .
[57] E. Newport,et al. When learners surpass their models: The acquisition of American Sign Language from inconsistent input , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.
[58] Lisa M. Oakes,et al. Early Category and Concept Development: Making Sense of the Blooming, Buzzing Confusion , 2008 .
[59] J. Weissenborn,et al. Functional Elements in Infants' Speech Processing: The Role of Determiners in the Syntactic Categorization of Lexical Elements , 2004 .
[60] L. Gerken,et al. Infants can use distributional cues to form syntactic categories , 2005, Journal of Child Language.
[61] Gary Lupyan,et al. Case, Word Order, and Language Learnability: Insights from Connectionist Modeling , 2019, Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.