Human Neuroscience

When acting jointly with others, adults can be as proficient as when acting individually. However, how young children coordinate their actions with another person and how their action coordination develops during early childhood is not well understood. By means of a sequential button-pressing game, which could be played jointly or individually, the action coordination of 2½- and 3-year-old children was examined. Performance accuracy and variability of response timing were taken as indicators of coordination ability. Results showed substantial improvement in joint action coordination between the age of 2½ and 3, but both age groups performed equally well when acting individually. Interestingly, 3-year-olds performed equally well in the joint and the individual condition, whereas 2½-year-olds did not yet show this adult-like pattern as indicated by less accurate performance in the joint action. The findings suggest that in contrast to 3-year-olds, 2½-year-olds still have difficulties in establishing well-coordinated joint action with an adult partner. Possible underlying cognitive abilities such as action planning and action control are discussed.

[1]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Helping and Cooperation at 14 Months of Age. , 2007, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[2]  K. Newell,et al.  Noise, variability, and the development of children’s perceptual-motor skills , 2005 .

[3]  G. Knoblich,et al.  Action coordination in groups and individuals: learning anticipatory control. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  Claes von Hofsten,et al.  Action in development. , 2007, Developmental science.

[5]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Celia A. Brownell,et al.  Changes in cooperation and self-other differentiation during the second year. , 1990, Child development.

[7]  Peter K. Smith A longitudinal study of social participation in preschool children: Solitary and parallel play reexamined. , 1978 .

[8]  Sarah Cummins-Sebree,et al.  The production of bimanual percussion in 12- to 24-month-old children. , 2007, Infant behavior & development.

[9]  C. Moore,et al.  The Cognitive Basis of Future-oriented Prosocial Behavior , 2001 .

[10]  Raymond H. Cuijpers,et al.  Joint Action: Neurocognitive Mechanisms Supporting Human Interaction , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[11]  Jane E. Clark,et al.  A longitudinal study of intralimb coordination in the first year of independent walking: a dynamical systems analysis. , 1993, Child development.

[12]  C. Eckerman,et al.  Toddlers' emerging ways of achieving social coordinations with a peer. , 1989, Child development.

[13]  Claes von Hofsten,et al.  Action in development. , 2007 .

[14]  S. J. Phillips,et al.  Human interlimb coordination: the first 6 months of independent walking. , 1988, Developmental psychobiology.

[15]  S. Bryson,et al.  Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. , 2008, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  J. Piek The role of variability in early motor development , 2002 .

[17]  Mary Gauvain,et al.  The social context of cognitive development , 2000 .

[18]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Your task is my task. Shared task representations in dyadic interactions , 2003 .

[19]  Wen-Jui Kuo,et al.  Action Co-representation is Tuned to Other Humans , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Flavell,et al.  Cognitive development: children's knowledge about the mind. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.

[21]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. , 2006, Child development.

[22]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Cooperative Problem‐Solving and Teaching in Preschoolers , 2001 .

[23]  N. Getchell Age and task-related differences in timing stability, consistency, and natural frequency of children's rhythmic, motor coordination. , 2006, Developmental psychobiology.

[24]  Knut Drewing,et al.  Sensorimotor synchronization across the life span , 2002 .

[25]  E. Markman,et al.  Prior experiences and perceived efficacy influence 3-year-olds' imitation. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[26]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Becoming a social partner with peers: cooperation and social understanding in one- and two-year-olds. , 2006, Child development.

[27]  Paul van Geert,et al.  Focus on variability: New tools to study intra-individual variability in developmental data , 2002 .

[28]  The Association between Intention Understanding and Peer Cooperation in Toddlers. , 2009 .

[29]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children's understanding of joint commitments. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[30]  A. Diamond Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. , 2002 .

[31]  Wen-Jui Kuo,et al.  A common coding framework in self–other interaction: evidence from joint action task , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.