Plans or Outcomes: How Do We Attribute Intelligence to Others?

Humans routinely make inferences about both the contents and the workings of other minds based on observed actions. People consider what others want or know, but also how intelligent, rational, or attentive they might be. Here, we introduce a new methodology for quantitatively studying the mechanisms people use to attribute intelligence to others based on their behavior. We focus on two key judgments previously proposed in the literature: judgments based on observed outcomes (you're smart if you won the game) and judgments based on evaluating the quality of an agent's planning that led to their outcomes (you're smart if you made the right choice, even if you didn't succeed). We present a novel task, the maze search task (MST), in which participants rate the intelligence of agents searching a maze for a hidden goal. We model outcome-based attributions based on the observed utility of the agent upon achieving a goal, with higher utilities indicating higher intelligence, and model planning-based attributions by measuring the proximity of the observed actions to an ideal planner, such that agents who produce closer approximations of optimal plans are seen as more intelligent. We examine human attributions of intelligence in three experiments that use MST and find that participants used both outcome and planning as indicators of intelligence. However, observing the outcome was not necessary, and participants still made planning-based attributions of intelligence when the outcome was not observed. We also found that the weights individuals placed on plans and on outcome correlated with an individual's ability to engage in cognitive reflection. Our results suggest that people attribute intelligence based on plans given sufficient context and cognitive resources and rely on the outcome when computational resources or context are limited.

[1]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  The Naïve Utility Calculus: Computational Principles Underlying Commonsense Psychology , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[3]  Joshua D. Greene,et al.  Finding faults: How moral dilemmas illuminate cognitive structure , 2012, Social neuroscience.

[4]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Ten-month-old infants infer the value of goals from the costs of actions , 2017, Science.

[5]  S. Asch Forming impressions of personality. , 1946, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[6]  V. McGeer The Regulative Dimension of Folk Psychology , 2007 .

[7]  Scott T. Allison,et al.  Outcome-biased inferences and the perception of change in groups , 1990 .

[8]  F. Heider,et al.  An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .

[9]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  Wouter Kool,et al.  Planning Complexity Registers as a Cost in Metacontrol , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  S. Frederick Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .

[12]  A. Todorov,et al.  Inferences of Competence from Faces Predict Election Outcomes , 2005, Science.

[13]  Mahzarin R. Banaji,et al.  The Role of Forgetting in Undermining Good Intentions , 2013, PloS one.

[14]  Jesse Chandler,et al.  Nonnaïveté among Amazon Mechanical Turk workers: Consequences and solutions for behavioral researchers , 2013, Behavior Research Methods.

[15]  P. Dayan,et al.  Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.

[16]  M. Lerner,et al.  The Belief in a Just World , 1980 .

[17]  R. Boyd A Different Kind of Animal , 2018 .

[18]  Justin W. Martin,et al.  Forthcoming: The Blackwell Companion to Experimental Philosophy The adaptive logic of moral luck , 2016 .

[19]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Lucky or clever? From expectations to responsibility judgments , 2018, Cognition.

[20]  J. Baron,et al.  Outcome bias in decision evaluation. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  Zoltan Kekecs,et al.  What is stupid? People's conception of unintelligent behavior , 2015 .

[22]  K L Posner,et al.  Effect of outcome on physician judgments of appropriateness of care. , 1991, JAMA.

[23]  Shane Legg,et al.  Universal Intelligence: A Definition of Machine Intelligence , 2007, Minds and Machines.

[24]  D. Gilbert,et al.  The correspondence bias. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[26]  Tadeusz W. Zawidzki Mindshaping , 2018, The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition.

[27]  H. Grice Logic and conversation , 1975 .

[28]  C. Surber Inferences of ability and effort: Evidence for two different processes. , 1984 .

[29]  B. Weiner,et al.  Necessary versus Sufficient Causal Schemata for Success and Failure. , 1973 .

[30]  Yarrow Dunham,et al.  Judgments of the Lucky across Development and Culture , 2022 .

[31]  K. Stanovich,et al.  The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[32]  Chris L. Baker,et al.  Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizing , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.

[33]  G. Fiorito,et al.  Grow Smart and Die Young: Why Did Cephalopods Evolve Intelligence? , 2019, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[34]  N. Ambady,et al.  Half a minute: Predicting teacher evaluations from thin slices of nonverbal behavior and physical attractiveness. , 1993 .

[35]  S. Johnson,et al.  Optimality Bias in Moral Judgment , 2017, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.

[36]  Brian J Scholl,et al.  Chasing vs. stalking: interrupting the perception of animacy. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  Steven A. Cholewiak,et al.  Agency and rationality: Adopting the intentional stance toward evolved virtual agents , 2016 .

[38]  The effect of outcome information on doctors' evaluations of their own diagnostic decisions , 2004, Medical education.

[39]  Bogdan Wojciszke,et al.  On the Dominance of Moral Categories in Impression Formation , 1998 .

[40]  P. Brickman,et al.  Consistency Versus Discrepancy as Clues in the Attribution of Intelligence and Motivation. , 1971 .

[41]  Tanya Behne,et al.  The Rationality of (Over)imitation , 2018, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[42]  F. Cushman,et al.  Expertise in Moral Reasoning? Order Effects on Moral Judgment in Professional Philosophers and Non-Philosophers , 2012 .

[43]  Daeyeol Lee Birth of Intelligence , 2020 .

[44]  L. Rips,et al.  Do the right thing: The assumption of optimality in lay decision theory and causal judgment , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.