Resolve, Capabilities, and the Outcomes of Interstate Disputes, 1816-1976

This study attempts to explain the recurring empirical observation that initiators of interstate disputes, both those that escalated into wars and those that ended otherwise, are disproportionately likely to win. Two competing models are developed: (1) The capability model argues that the outcome of the dispute is a function of the balance of capabilities among the protagonists and that the strongest prevails. (2) The resolve model argues that dispute outcomes are determined by the balance of motivation and resolve among the protagonists; that is, the actor with the highest level of resolve is likely to win. A random sample of 164 disputes is used to test these two models. The findings suggest that capabilities are unrelated to dispute outcomes, whereas resolve variables are consistently related to dispute outcomes. The implications for conflict management and conflict escalation are discussed.

[1]  James D. Laing,et al.  Prediction Analysis of Cross Classifications. , 1976 .

[2]  John H. Aldrich,et al.  Probing the Bounds of Conventional Wisdom. A Comparison of Regression, Probit, and Discriminant Analysis* , 1975 .

[3]  N. Miller,et al.  Frustration and aggression , 1939 .

[4]  J. David Singer,et al.  Quantitative international politics , 1968 .

[5]  Moshe Dayan,et al.  Diary Of The Sinai Campaign , 1967 .

[6]  A. George The Limits Of Coercive Diplomacy , 1971 .

[7]  K. Holsti Resolving international conflicts: a taxonomy of behavior and some figures on procedures , 1966 .

[8]  Michael Brecher,et al.  Decisions in Israel's foreign policy , 1975 .

[9]  Bruce Russett,et al.  Peace, war, and numbers , 1972 .

[10]  Roy L. Behr,et al.  Nice Guys Finish Last—Sometimes , 1981 .

[11]  T. Schelling The Strategy of Conflict , 1963 .

[12]  Y. Harkabi Nuclear war and nuclear peace , 1966 .

[13]  Michael Brecher,et al.  Toward a Theory of International Crisis BehaviorA Preliminary Report , 1977 .

[14]  Richard Smoke,et al.  Deterrence in American Foreign Policy , 1974 .

[15]  Peter Burke,et al.  Log-linear models , 1980 .

[16]  O. Holsti Crisis, escalation, war , 1972 .

[17]  R. Axelrod More Effective Choice in the Prisoner's Dilemma , 1980 .

[18]  G Dean,et al.  Theory and Research on the Causes of War , 1969 .

[19]  Moderation from management : international organizations and peace , 1978 .

[20]  R. McKelvey,et al.  A statistical model for the analysis of ordinal level dependent variables , 1975 .

[21]  William L. Shirer The Rise and Fall of the Third Reich , 1960 .

[22]  J. Kugler,et al.  The War Ledger , 2021, Rebellion, Reconstruction, and Redemption, 1861–1893.

[23]  L. Berkowitz The Concept of Aggressive Drive: Some Additional Considerations1 , 1965 .

[24]  Leonard Berkowitz,et al.  The Frustration-Aggression Hypothesis Revisited , 1969 .

[25]  Barry M. Blechman,et al.  Force without War: U.S. Armed Forces as a Political Instrument , 1978 .

[26]  Michael D. Wallace Armaments and Escalation , 1982 .

[27]  Jack Snyder Rationality at the Brink: The Role of Cognitive Processes in Failures of Deterrence , 1978, World Politics.

[28]  J. Hart Three approaches to the measurement of power in international relations , 1976, International Organization.

[29]  S. Maxwell Rationality in Deterrence , 1968 .

[30]  Bruce Bueno de Mesquita,et al.  Choosing Sides in Wars , 1979 .

[31]  Bruce Bueno de Mesquita,et al.  The War Trap , 1981 .

[32]  S. Hoffmann The state of war , 1965 .

[33]  T. Gurr,et al.  Why Men Rebel. , 1970 .

[34]  R. Axelrod Effective Choice in the Prisoner's Dilemma , 1980 .

[35]  Robert Axelrod,et al.  The Rational Timing of Surprise , 1979, World Politics.

[36]  Chadwick F. Alger,et al.  Quantitative International Politics: Insights and Evidence , 1968 .

[37]  J. David Singer,et al.  Correlates of War , 2008, Encyclopedia of Violence, Peace, & Conflict.

[38]  R. Axelrod The Emergence of Cooperation among Egoists , 1981, American Political Science Review.