Events by Hands and Feet

The human mind carves time into events much as it carves space into objects. Events are activities that are perceived to have beginnings, middles, and ends, such as going to work and making a bed. Events performed by humans can be enacted by feet, as in getting to work, or by hands, as in making a bed. Although continuous, events are perceived to have discrete parts. Events by feet are segmented into actions at nodes, or turns at landmarks, as revealed in spontaneously produced route maps and route directions. In contrast, events by hands are segmented hierarchically. At the coarse level, the segments are punctuated by objects or object parts, sheets, pillowcases, and blanket in the case of making the bed. At the fine level, segments are punctuated by articulated actions on the same object, spreading the sheet, tucking in the corners, smoothing it out. For both events by feet and events by hands, the segments correspond to changes in goals in subgoals, signaled by perceptually salient changes in physical activity.

[1]  Savas Tsohatsidis Meanings and prototypes : studies in linguistic categorization , 1990 .

[2]  A. Rifkin,et al.  Evidence for a basic level in event taxonomies , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[3]  E. Spelke,et al.  Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. , 1995 .

[4]  Paul U. Lee,et al.  Pictorial and Verbal Tools for Conveying Routes , 1999, COSIT.

[5]  Daniel R. Montello,et al.  Elements of Good Route Directions in Familiar and Unfamiliar Environments , 1999, COSIT.

[6]  Darren Newtson Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior. , 1973 .

[7]  H. Couclelis VERBAL DIRECTIONS FOR WAY-FINDING: SPACE, COGNITION, AND LANGUAGE , 1996 .

[8]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Distinguishing Between Animates And Inanimates: Not By Motion Alone , 1995 .

[9]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[11]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  G. Allen Principles and practices for communicating route knowledge , 2000 .

[13]  B. Tversky,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : General VOL . 113 , No . 2 JUNE 1984 Objects , Parts , and Categories , 2005 .

[14]  Jodie A. Baird,et al.  Infants parse dynamic action. , 2001, Child development.

[15]  M. Denis,et al.  Spatial discourse and navigation: an analysis of route directions in the city of Venice , 1999 .

[16]  G. Murphy,et al.  Converging operations on a basic level in event taxonomies , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[17]  Roberto Casati,et al.  Parts And Places , 1999 .

[18]  M. Denis The description of routes : A cognitive approach to the production of spatial discourse , 1997 .

[19]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Event structure in perception and conception. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  R. Byrne Imitation without intentionality. Using string parsing to copy the organization of behaviour , 1999, Animal Cognition.

[21]  M. Denis,et al.  Language and spatial cognition: comparing the roles of landmarks and street names in route instructions , 2004 .

[22]  B. Tversky,et al.  The Imitative Mind: On bodies and events , 2002 .

[23]  A. Tversky,et al.  Representations of perceptions of risks , 1984 .