Neural correlates of deception in social contexts in normally developing children

Deception is related to the ability to inhibit prepotent responses and to engage in mental tasks such as anticipating responses and inferring what another person knows, especially in social contexts. However, the neural correlates of deception processing, which requires mentalizing, remain unclear. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we examined the neural correlates of deception, including mentalization, in social contexts in normally developing children. Healthy right-handed children (aged 8–9 years) were scanned while performing interactive games involving deception. The games varied along two dimensions: the type of reply (deception and truth) and the type of context (social and less social). Participants were instructed to deceive a witch and to tell the truth to a girl. Under the social-context conditions, participants were asked to consider what they inferred about protagonists' preferences from their facial expressions when responding to questions. Under the less-social-context conditions, participants did not need to consider others' preferences. We found a significantly greater response in the right precuneus under the social-context than under less-social-context conditions. Additionally, we found marginally greater activation in the right inferior parietal lobule (IPL) under the deception than under the truth condition. These results suggest that deception in a social context requires not only inhibition of prepotent responses but also engagement in mentalizing processes. This study provides the first evidence of the neural correlates of the mentalizing processes involved in deception in normally developing children.

[1]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Neural Correlates of Online Action Preparation , 2023, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  R. Kawashima,et al.  Correlation between gray matter density‐adjusted brain perfusion and age using brain MR images of 202 healthy children , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[3]  Angela D. Evans,et al.  Exploring the Ability to Deceive in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2011, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[4]  Serge A R B Rombouts,et al.  Changing Brains, Changing Perspectives , 2011, Psychological science.

[5]  B. Sodian,et al.  Modulation of the cortical false belief network during development , 2010, Brain Research.

[6]  T. Lyon,et al.  Young children's emerging ability to make false statements. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[7]  H. Heekeren,et al.  Sociotopy in the temporoparietal cortex: common versus distinct processes. , 2010, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[8]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural processes underlying self- and other-related lies: An individual difference approach using fMRI , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[9]  Xinghua Lu,et al.  Functional MRI Detection of Deception After Committing a Mock Sabotage Crime * , 2009, Journal of forensic sciences.

[10]  R. Gur,et al.  fMRI investigation of the cognitive structure of the Concealed Information Test , 2008, Neurocase.

[11]  G. Juckel,et al.  Cooperation and Deception Recruit Different Subsets of the Theory-of-Mind Network , 2008, PloS one.

[12]  Justin H G Williams,et al.  Self–other relations in social development and autism: multiple roles for mirror neurons and other brain bases , 2008, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[13]  M. Tsukada,et al.  Neural Correlates of True Memory, False Memory, and Deception , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  C. Frith,et al.  Detecting deception: the scope and limits , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  J. Pekar,et al.  Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  P. Stoeter,et al.  Covariations among fMRI, skin conductance, and behavioral data during processing of concealed information , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[17]  C. Frith,et al.  Social Cognition in Humans , 2007, Current Biology.

[18]  M. Brammer,et al.  Progressive increase of frontostriatal brain activation from childhood to adulthood during event‐related tasks of cognitive control , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[19]  Mirella Dapretto,et al.  Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[20]  R. Saxe Uniquely human social cognition , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[21]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  E. Mori,et al.  Dissociable roles of prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in deception. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  S. Platek,et al.  Brain mapping of deception and truth telling about an ecologically valid situation: functional MR imaging and polygraph investigation--initial experience. , 2006, Radiology.

[25]  R. Gur,et al.  Telling truth from lie in individual subjects with fast event‐related fMRI , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[26]  Kevin A. Johnson,et al.  Detecting Deception Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[27]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  The role of precuneus and left inferior frontal cortex during source memory episodic retrieval , 2005, NeuroImage.

[28]  C. Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2005, Current Biology.

[29]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Intentional false responding shares neural substrates with response conflict and cognitive control , 2005, NeuroImage.

[30]  G. McCarthy,et al.  When Strangers Pass , 2004, Psychological science.

[31]  Kiyotaka Nemoto,et al.  The neural network for the mirror system and mentalizing in normally developed children: an fMRI study , 2004, Neuroreport.

[32]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[33]  James R. Booth,et al.  Neural development of selective attention and response inhibition , 2003, NeuroImage.

[34]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  The Feasibility of a Common Stereotactic Space for Children and Adults in fMRI Studies of Development , 2002, NeuroImage.

[35]  Chetwyn C. H. Chan,et al.  Lie detection by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[36]  R. C. Gur,et al.  Brain Activity during Simulated Deception: An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[37]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Immature Frontal Lobe Contributions to Cognitive Control in Children Evidence from fMRI , 2002, Neuron.

[38]  I. Wilkinson,et al.  Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans , 2001, Neuroreport.

[39]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[40]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.

[41]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Human Brain Function , 1997 .

[43]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[44]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Face encoding and recognition in the human brain. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[46]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Age-related reductions in human recognition memory due to impaired encoding. , 1995, Science.

[47]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Brain regions associated with acquisition and retrieval of verbal episodic memory , 1994, Nature.

[48]  F. Happé An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[49]  S. Baron-Cohen Out of sight or out of mind? Another look at deception in autism. , 1992, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[50]  U. Frith,et al.  Deception and sabotage in autistic, retarded and normal children. , 1992, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[51]  J. Morton,et al.  The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: autism , 1991, Trends in Neurosciences.

[52]  Josef Perner,et al.  Early Deception and the Child's Theory of Mind: False Trails and Genuine Markers , 1991 .

[53]  A. Leslie,et al.  Autistic children's understanding of seeing, knowing and believing , 1988 .

[54]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[55]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescents and adults. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[57]  Bregtje Gunther Moor,et al.  Neurodevelopmental changes of reading the mind in the eyes. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[58]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial transformation of images , 1997 .

[59]  N. Yirmiya,et al.  The Ability to Manipulate Behavior and to Understand Manipulation of Beliefs: A Comparison of Individuals with Autism, Mental Retardation, and Normal Development. , 1996 .

[60]  J. Peskin Ruse and Representations: On Children's Ability to Conceal Information. , 1992 .

[61]  Thomas Baumgartner,et al.  Article the Neural Circuitry of a Broken Promise , 2022 .