P300 amplitude is determined by target-to-target interval.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] H. Davis,et al. The slow response of the human cortex to auditory stimuli: recovery process. , 1966, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[2] D. Kahneman,et al. Attention and Effort , 1973 .
[3] Steven W. Keele,et al. Attention and human performance , 1973 .
[4] N. Squires,et al. The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. , 1976, Science.
[5] J. Ford,et al. Parameters of temporal recovery of the human auditory evoked potential. , 1976, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[6] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. The effects of stimulus sequence on event related potentials: A comparison of visual and auditory sequences , 1977 .
[7] S A Hillyard,et al. P3 waves to the discrimination of targets in homogeneous and heterogeneous stimulus sequences. , 1977, Psychophysiology.
[8] E Courchesne,et al. Changes in P3 waves with event repetition: long-term effects on scalp distribution and amplitude. , 1978, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[9] T W Picton,et al. The component structure of the human event-related potentials. , 1980, Progress in brain research.
[10] D L Woods,et al. Electrophysiological signs of split-second decision-making. , 1980, Science.
[11] D L Woods,et al. Recovery cycles of event-related potentials in multiple detection tasks. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[12] C D Wickens,et al. The Event-Related Brain Potential as an Index of Display-Monitoring Workload , 1980, Human factors.
[13] E Donchin,et al. P300 and stimulus categorization: two plus one is not so different from one plus one. , 1980, Psychophysiology.
[14] W. Pritchard. Psychophysiology of P300. , 1981, Psychological bulletin.
[15] T W Picton,et al. Temporal and sequential probability in evoked potential studies. , 1981, Canadian journal of psychology.
[16] Ernst Fernando Lopes Da Silva Niedermeyer,et al. Electroencephalography, basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields , 1982 .
[17] A Pfefferbaum,et al. Expectancy for events in old age: stimulus sequence effects on P300 and reaction time. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.
[18] E Donchin,et al. Sequential expectancies and decision making in a changing environment: an electrophysiological approach. , 1982, Psychophysiology.
[19] Emanuel Donchin,et al. The P300 component of the event-related brain potential as an index of information processing , 1982, Biological Psychology.
[20] R. Näätänen,et al. Sequential effects on the ERP in discriminating two stimuli , 1983, Biological Psychology.
[21] E. Donchin,et al. Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. , 1983, Science.
[22] T. Picton,et al. The Effects of Probability and Discriminability on the Evoked Potentials to Unpredictable Stimuli a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[23] B S Kopell,et al. Effects of Stimulus Sequence on P300 and Reaction Time in Schizophrenics , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[24] C D Wickens,et al. Processing of stimulus properties: evidence for dual-task integrality. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] A. D. Fisk,et al. The effects of practice and task structure on components of the event-related brain potential. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[26] H. Semlitsch,et al. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[27] E. Donchin,et al. Cognitive Psychophysiology and Human Information Processing , 1986 .
[28] R. Johnson. A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[29] D L Woods,et al. The recovery functions of auditory event-related potentials during split-second discriminations. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[30] Rolf Verleger,et al. Sequential effects on P3 in a counting task: A partial replication , 1987, Biological Psychology.
[31] J. Polich,et al. Task difficulty, probability, and inter-stimulus interval as determinants of P300 from auditory stimuli. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[32] D. Ruchkin,et al. Functional differences between members of the P300 complex: P3e and P3b. , 1987, Psychophysiology.
[33] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[34] J. Polich,et al. Long latency auditory evoked potentials: intensity, inter-stimulus interval, and habituation. , 1988, The Pavlovian journal of biological science.
[35] W. Sommer,et al. Consciousness of attention and expectancy as reflected in event-related potentials and reaction times. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[36] F E Bloom,et al. P300 assessment of early Alzheimer's disease. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[37] J. Polich. Probability and inter-stimulus interval effects on the P300 from auditory stimuli. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[38] J. Polich,et al. P300, probability, and interstimulus interval. , 1990, Psychophysiology.
[39] Christoph Braun,et al. Auditory and somatosensory event-related potentials: I. Effects of attention , 1991 .
[40] R. Verleger,et al. The instruction to refrain from blinking affects auditory P3 and N1 amplitudes. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[41] R Verleger,et al. The waltzing oddball. , 1991, Psychophysiology.
[42] W. Sommer,et al. Differential effects of voluntary expectancies on reaction times and event-related potentials: evidence for automatic and controlled expectancies. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[43] J. Polich,et al. P300, global probability, and stimulus sequence effects in children , 1992 .
[44] T W Picton,et al. The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.
[45] W. Sommer,et al. Stimulus presentation rate dissociates sequential effects in event-related potentials and reaction times. , 1993, Psychophysiology.
[46] G. A. Miller,et al. Memory template comparison processes in anhedonia and dysthymia. , 1993, Psychophysiology.
[47] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[48] J. Polich,et al. P300 from a single auditory stimulus. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[49] R. Oades,et al. The topography of event-related potentials in passive and active conditions of a 3-tone auditory oddball test. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.
[50] Evian Gordon,et al. Numbers of preceding nontargets differentially affect responses to targets in normal volunteers and patients with schizophrenia: A study of event-related potentials , 1995, Psychiatry Research.
[51] J. Polich,et al. P300, probability, and the three-tone paradigm. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[52] J. Polich,et al. P300 topography and modality effects from a single-stimulus paradigm. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[53] J. Polich,et al. P300 from one-, two-, and three-stimulus auditory paradigms. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[54] G A Miller,et al. How we think about cognition, emotion, and biology in psychopathology. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[55] J. Cohen,et al. P300, stimulus intensity, modality, and probability. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[56] J. Polich,et al. P300 and probability: comparison of oddball and single-stimulus paradigms. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[57] John Polich,et al. P300 from a single-stimulus paradigm: auditory intensity and tone frequency effects , 1997, Biological Psychology.
[58] J. Polich,et al. P300 from a single-stimulus paradigm: passive versus active tasks and stimulus modality. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[59] John Polich,et al. P300 Sequence Effects, Probability, and Interstimulus Interval , 1997, Physiology & Behavior.
[60] J. Polich. P300 clinical utility and control of variability. , 1998, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.
[61] J. Polich,et al. Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[62] Werner Sommer,et al. The expectancies that govern the P300 amplitude are mostly automatic and unconscious , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[63] E Gordon,et al. Is the target-to-target interval a critical determinant of P3 amplitude? , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[64] E. Soetens,et al. Covert signs of expectancy in serial reaction time tasks revealed by event-related potentials , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[65] J. Polich,et al. Comparison of P300 from passive and active tasks for auditory and visual stimuli. , 1999, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[66] P300 sequence effects differ between children and adults for auditory stimuli. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[67] Ina M. Tarkka,et al. Source Localization of P300 from Oddball, Single Stimulus, and Omitted-Stimulus Paradigms , 2004, Brain Topography.