P300 amplitude is determined by target-to-target interval.

P300 event-related brain potential (ERP) measures are affected by target stimulus probability, the number of nontargets preceding the target in the stimulus sequence structure, and interstimulus interval (ISI). Each of these factors contributes to the target-to-target interval (TTI), which also has been found to affect P300. The present study employed a variant of the oddball paradigm and manipulated the number of preceding nontarget stimuli (0, 1, 2, 3) and ISI (1, 2, 4 s) in order to systematically assess TTI effects on P300 values from auditory and visual stimuli. Number of preceding nontargets generally produced stronger effects than ISI in a manner suggesting that TTI determined P300 measures: Amplitude increased as TTI increased for both auditory and visual stimulus conditions, whereas latency tended to decrease with increased TTI. The finding that TTI is a critical determinant of P300 responsivity is discussed within a resource allocation theoretical framework.

[1]  H. Davis,et al.  The slow response of the human cortex to auditory stimuli: recovery process. , 1966, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[2]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[3]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Attention and human performance , 1973 .

[4]  N. Squires,et al.  The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. , 1976, Science.

[5]  J. Ford,et al.  Parameters of temporal recovery of the human auditory evoked potential. , 1976, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[6]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  The effects of stimulus sequence on event related potentials: A comparison of visual and auditory sequences , 1977 .

[7]  S A Hillyard,et al.  P3 waves to the discrimination of targets in homogeneous and heterogeneous stimulus sequences. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[8]  E Courchesne,et al.  Changes in P3 waves with event repetition: long-term effects on scalp distribution and amplitude. , 1978, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[9]  T W Picton,et al.  The component structure of the human event-related potentials. , 1980, Progress in brain research.

[10]  D L Woods,et al.  Electrophysiological signs of split-second decision-making. , 1980, Science.

[11]  D L Woods,et al.  Recovery cycles of event-related potentials in multiple detection tasks. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[12]  C D Wickens,et al.  The Event-Related Brain Potential as an Index of Display-Monitoring Workload , 1980, Human factors.

[13]  E Donchin,et al.  P300 and stimulus categorization: two plus one is not so different from one plus one. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[14]  W. Pritchard Psychophysiology of P300. , 1981, Psychological bulletin.

[15]  T W Picton,et al.  Temporal and sequential probability in evoked potential studies. , 1981, Canadian journal of psychology.

[16]  Ernst Fernando Lopes Da Silva Niedermeyer,et al.  Electroencephalography, basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields , 1982 .

[17]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  Expectancy for events in old age: stimulus sequence effects on P300 and reaction time. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.

[18]  E Donchin,et al.  Sequential expectancies and decision making in a changing environment: an electrophysiological approach. , 1982, Psychophysiology.

[19]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  The P300 component of the event-related brain potential as an index of information processing , 1982, Biological Psychology.

[20]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Sequential effects on the ERP in discriminating two stimuli , 1983, Biological Psychology.

[21]  E. Donchin,et al.  Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. , 1983, Science.

[22]  T. Picton,et al.  The Effects of Probability and Discriminability on the Evoked Potentials to Unpredictable Stimuli a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[23]  B S Kopell,et al.  Effects of Stimulus Sequence on P300 and Reaction Time in Schizophrenics , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  C D Wickens,et al.  Processing of stimulus properties: evidence for dual-task integrality. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  A. D. Fisk,et al.  The effects of practice and task structure on components of the event-related brain potential. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[26]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[27]  E. Donchin,et al.  Cognitive Psychophysiology and Human Information Processing , 1986 .

[28]  R. Johnson A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[29]  D L Woods,et al.  The recovery functions of auditory event-related potentials during split-second discriminations. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[30]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Sequential effects on P3 in a counting task: A partial replication , 1987, Biological Psychology.

[31]  J. Polich,et al.  Task difficulty, probability, and inter-stimulus interval as determinants of P300 from auditory stimuli. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[32]  D. Ruchkin,et al.  Functional differences between members of the P300 complex: P3e and P3b. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[33]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[34]  J. Polich,et al.  Long latency auditory evoked potentials: intensity, inter-stimulus interval, and habituation. , 1988, The Pavlovian journal of biological science.

[35]  W. Sommer,et al.  Consciousness of attention and expectancy as reflected in event-related potentials and reaction times. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  F E Bloom,et al.  P300 assessment of early Alzheimer's disease. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  J. Polich Probability and inter-stimulus interval effects on the P300 from auditory stimuli. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[38]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, probability, and interstimulus interval. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[39]  Christoph Braun,et al.  Auditory and somatosensory event-related potentials: I. Effects of attention , 1991 .

[40]  R. Verleger,et al.  The instruction to refrain from blinking affects auditory P3 and N1 amplitudes. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[41]  R Verleger,et al.  The waltzing oddball. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[42]  W. Sommer,et al.  Differential effects of voluntary expectancies on reaction times and event-related potentials: evidence for automatic and controlled expectancies. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, global probability, and stimulus sequence effects in children , 1992 .

[44]  T W Picton,et al.  The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[45]  W. Sommer,et al.  Stimulus presentation rate dissociates sequential effects in event-related potentials and reaction times. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[46]  G. A. Miller,et al.  Memory template comparison processes in anhedonia and dysthymia. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[47]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[48]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 from a single auditory stimulus. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[49]  R. Oades,et al.  The topography of event-related potentials in passive and active conditions of a 3-tone auditory oddball test. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.

[50]  Evian Gordon,et al.  Numbers of preceding nontargets differentially affect responses to targets in normal volunteers and patients with schizophrenia: A study of event-related potentials , 1995, Psychiatry Research.

[51]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, probability, and the three-tone paradigm. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[52]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 topography and modality effects from a single-stimulus paradigm. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[53]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 from one-, two-, and three-stimulus auditory paradigms. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[54]  G A Miller,et al.  How we think about cognition, emotion, and biology in psychopathology. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[55]  J. Cohen,et al.  P300, stimulus intensity, modality, and probability. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[56]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 and probability: comparison of oddball and single-stimulus paradigms. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[57]  John Polich,et al.  P300 from a single-stimulus paradigm: auditory intensity and tone frequency effects , 1997, Biological Psychology.

[58]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 from a single-stimulus paradigm: passive versus active tasks and stimulus modality. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[59]  John Polich,et al.  P300 Sequence Effects, Probability, and Interstimulus Interval , 1997, Physiology & Behavior.

[60]  J. Polich P300 clinical utility and control of variability. , 1998, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[61]  J. Polich,et al.  Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[62]  Werner Sommer,et al.  The expectancies that govern the P300 amplitude are mostly automatic and unconscious , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[63]  E Gordon,et al.  Is the target-to-target interval a critical determinant of P3 amplitude? , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[64]  E. Soetens,et al.  Covert signs of expectancy in serial reaction time tasks revealed by event-related potentials , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[65]  J. Polich,et al.  Comparison of P300 from passive and active tasks for auditory and visual stimuli. , 1999, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[66]  P300 sequence effects differ between children and adults for auditory stimuli. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[67]  Ina M. Tarkka,et al.  Source Localization of P300 from Oddball, Single Stimulus, and Omitted-Stimulus Paradigms , 2004, Brain Topography.