Animals' use of landmarks and metric information to reorient: effects of the size of the experimental space
暂无分享,去创建一个
Valeria Anna Sovrano | Giorgio Vallortigara | Angelo Bisazza | G. Vallortigara | A. Bisazza | V. A. Sovrano
[1] E. Spelke,et al. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.
[2] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Modularity as a fish (Xenotoca eiseni) views it: conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial reorientation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[3] T. S. Collett,et al. Landmark learning and visuo-spatial memories in gerbils , 1986, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[4] G. Vallortigara,et al. Young chickens learn to localize the centre of a spatial environment , 1997, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[5] L. Pizzamiglio,et al. TENS modulates spatial reorientantion in neglect patients , 2000, Neuroreport.
[6] C. Gallistel. The organization of learning , 1990 .
[7] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Conjoining information from different modules: A comparative perspective , 2002 .
[8] Catherine Thinus-Blanc,et al. Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task , 2001 .
[9] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Visual Cognition and Representation in Birds and Primates , 2004 .
[10] Ken Cheng,et al. Use of Landmark Configuration in Pigeons and Humans: II. Generality Across Search Tasks , 1997 .
[11] E. Spelke,et al. Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .
[12] Edward J Golob,et al. Differences between appetitive and aversive reinforcement on reorientation in a spatial working memory task , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.
[13] G. Vallortigara,et al. Searching for the center: spatial cognition in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[14] K. Cheng. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.
[15] G. Vallortigara,et al. Lateralization of response to social stimuli in fishes: A comparison between different methods and species , 2001, Physiology & Behavior.
[16] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.
[17] Marc D. Hauser,et al. The role of landmarks in cotton-top tamarin spatial foraging: evidence for geometric and non-geometric features , 2001, Animal Cognition.
[18] Lynn Nadel,et al. Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.
[19] Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al. What makes us smart? Core knowledge and natural language , 2003 .
[20] Marcia L. Spetch,et al. Searching in the center: pigeons (Columba livid) encode relative distance from walls of an enclosure. , 2004, Journal of comparative psychology.
[21] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish , 2002, Cognition.
[22] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Separate Geometric and Non-Geometric Modules for Spatial Reorientation: Evidence from a Lopsided Animal Brain , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[23] Lesley J. Rogers,et al. Comparative vertebrate cognition : are primates superior to non-primates? , 2004 .
[24] Marcia L. Spetch,et al. Pigeons encode relative geometry. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[25] Debbie M. Kelly,et al. Pigeons' (Columba livia) encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. , 1998 .
[26] N. Newcombe,et al. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[27] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: a test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). , 1990, Journal of comparative psychology.
[28] P. Carruthers. The cognitive functions of language , 2002, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[29] D. Gentner,et al. Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought , 2003 .
[30] M. Shatz. Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought (review) , 2006 .
[31] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[32] G. Vallortigara,et al. Roots of brain specializations: preferential left-eye use during mirror-image inspection in six species of teleost fish , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.
[33] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual-Task Studies of Space and Language , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.