Stereoacuity at Distance and Near

Purpose. Because previous studies have reported conflicting evidence, we examined a possible difference in stereoacuity between distance and near, in particular using a random-dot display. We compared distance and near stereoacuities using identical presentation formats at the two distances. Methods. Twelve young adults with low, stable refractive errors and apparently normal binocular vision participated. Stereoacuity was determined with a Mentor B-VAT II using Random Dot E (BVRDE) and Contour Circles (BVC) stereograms presented on a standard monitor (25 × 19.3 cm) at 518 cm (distance-habitual) and a small monitor (2.0 × 1.4 cm) at 40 cm (near-habitual). To examine whether accommodation-convergence influenced stereoacuity, testing at 40 cm was repeated with the addition of +2.50 DS lenses and base-in prisms (near-compensated) that created the same accommodation and convergence demands as for distance-habitual viewing. Results. The two stereotests produced similar findings. Stereoacuity was not significantly different for distance-habitual and near-habitual viewing of the BVRDE (p = 0.43) and BVC (p = 0.79) stereotests. Near-compensated stereoacuity was worse than near-habitual (BVRDE, p = 0.005; BVC, p = 0.004) and distance-habitual (BVRDE, p = 0.05; BVC, p = 0.003) for both stereotests. Because near stereoacuity with yoked prisms (control condition) was the same as without prism (near-habitual), prism-induced optical distortions cannot account for the difference. Conclusions. Stereoacuity was not different at distance and near under normal viewing conditions. The conflict between subject knowledge of target proximity and the optically-induced relaxation of accommodation and convergence, or an inaccurate accommodative-convergence response, might have caused poor near-compensated stereoacuity.

[1]  Daum M. Kent,et al.  Foundations of Binocular Vision: A Clinical Perspective. , 2001 .

[2]  E. Hita,et al.  Changes in Stereoscopic Depth Perception Caused by Decentration of Spectacle Lenses , 2000, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[3]  S B Kaye,et al.  Monocular and binocular depth discrimination thresholds. , 1999, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[4]  C. Rivadulla,et al.  Depth Perception in Random Dot Stereograms Is Not Affected by Changes in Either Vergence or Accommodation , 1998, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[5]  E. Peli,et al.  The visual effects of head-mounted display (HMD) are not distinguishable from those of desk-top computer display , 1998, Vision Research.

[6]  C Yildirim,et al.  Distance stereoacuity norms for the mentor B-VAT II-SG video acuity tester in young children and young adults. , 1998, Journal of AAPOS : the official publication of the American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus.

[7]  T. Fricke,et al.  Non‐stereoscopic cues in the Random‐Dot E stereotest: results for adult observers * , 1997, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[8]  F. A. Miles Binocular Vision and Stereopsis by Ian P. Howard and Brian J. Rogers, Oxford University Press, 1995. £90.00 (736 pages) ISBN 0 19 508476 4. , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[9]  S Thorpe,et al.  Modulation of neural stereoscopic processing in primate area V1 by the viewing distance. , 1992, Science.

[10]  A L Rosenbaum,et al.  A new method for evaluating distance stereo acuity. , 1991, Journal of pediatric ophthalmology and strabismus.

[11]  R. Manny,et al.  Testing stereopsis in the preschool child: is it clinically useful? , 1991, Journal of pediatric ophthalmology and strabismus.

[12]  J. Sivak Optometry and vision science. , 1989, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[13]  R. Garzia,et al.  Effect of Nearpoint Stress on the Horizontal Forced Vergence Fixation Disparity Curve , 1986, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.

[14]  R. Reading Binocular vision : foundations and applications , 1983 .

[15]  J. Sheedy Fixation disparity analysis of oculomotor imbalance. , 1980, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.

[16]  A Lit,et al.  Variability of depth-discrimination thresholds as a function of observation distance. , 1976, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[17]  J. Pokorny Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1972 .

[18]  L O Harvey,et al.  Critical flicker frequency as a function of viewing distance, stimulus size and luminance. , 1970, Vision research.

[19]  K. N. Ogle,et al.  Stereoscopic acuity and observation distance. , 1965, Investigative ophthalmology.

[20]  G AMIGO,et al.  Variation of stereoscopic acuity with observation distance. , 1963, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[21]  D JAMESON,et al.  Note on factors influencing the relation between stereoscopic acuity and observation distance. , 1959, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[22]  K N OGLE,et al.  Note on stereoscopic acuity and observation distance. , 1958, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[23]  G. Fry,et al.  The Effects Of Base-in And Base-out Prisms On Stereo-acuity , 1944 .

[24]  E. Freeman,et al.  Anomalies of Visual Acuity in Relation to Stimulus-Distance* , 1932 .