Are absolute and comparative judgements about road traffic-risks inherent in speeding realistic? A study among French traffic regulation offenders

Abstract A few studies have shown that road users can express realistic risk judgements, in the sense that they perceive higher risk when their behavior is in fact riskier. Our aim was to examine whether the realism of absolute and comparative judgements about speeding risks could be found among an at-risk driver population made of 539 French traffic regulation offenders taking a driving licence points recovery course. The results showed that course takers’ absolute judgements regarding speeding-ticket risk were realistic: the more course-takers had the intention to drive fast and reported driving-related sensation-seeking (DRSS), the more they evaluated themselves as being at risk of having a speeding-ticket. The realism was also observed when considering comparative judgements regarding speeding-ticket risk, but only among the more than 25 years old participants. Moreover, the realism was not observed when considering course takers’ absolute and comparative judgements regarding speeding-induced crash risk. Hence, it is important to increase communication about the dangerousness of speeding among specific groups such as traffic regulation offenders, above all among young course takers who reported high speeds and DRSS.

[1]  D. Parker,et al.  Extending the theory of planned behaviour: The role of personal norm , 1995 .

[2]  Wilma Otten,et al.  Context effects in the measurement of optimism in probability judgment , 1996 .

[3]  Mireille Chiron,et al.  Individuals taking a French driving licence points recovery course: their attitudes towards violations. , 2008, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[4]  Peter R. Harris,et al.  The impact of perceived experience on likelihood judgments for self and others: an experimental approach , 2007 .

[5]  Thierry Meyer Vulnérabilité subjective à l'influence des médias , 1995 .

[6]  Teresa O'Connor,et al.  Risk factors for fatal crashes in rural Australia. , 2011, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[7]  Kim S. Sankey,et al.  Relationships between young drivers' personality characteristics, risk perceptions, and driving behaviour. , 2008, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[8]  Torbjørn Rundmo,et al.  Cross-cultural comparisons of traffic safety, risk perception, attitudes and behaviour , 2009 .

[9]  Dana Yagil,et al.  Reasoned Action and Irrational Motives: A Prediction of Drivers' Intention to Violate Traffic Laws1 , 2001 .

[10]  W. Klein,et al.  Dispositional, Unrealistic, and Comparative Optimism: Differential Relations with the Knowledge and Processing of Risk Information and Beliefs about Personal Risk , 2002 .

[11]  R Fuller,et al.  Behavior analysis and unsafe driving: Warning-learning trap ahead! , 1991, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[12]  Lars Åberg,et al.  Stability in drivers' speed choice , 2002 .

[13]  Mireille Chiron,et al.  Who takes driving licence point recovery courses in France? Comparison between course-takers and ordinary drivers , 2010 .

[14]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  Replication and extension: The effect of the commitment to comply with speed limits in rehabilitation training courses for traffic regulation offenders in France , 2008 .

[15]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  Risk of crashing with a tram: Perceptions of pedestrians, cyclists, and motorists , 2012 .

[16]  F. McKenna,et al.  Factors underlying illusory self-assessment of driving skill in males and females. , 1991, Accident Analysis and Prevention.

[17]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  Are drivers' comparative risk judgments about speeding realistic? , 2009, Journal of safety research.

[18]  Richard Tay,et al.  Examining the effectiveness of physical threats in road safety advertising: The role of the third-person effect, gender, and age , 2007 .

[19]  P Delhomme,et al.  Comparing one's driving with others': assessment of abilities and frequency of offences. Evidence for a superior conformity of self-bias? , 1991, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[20]  J T Shope,et al.  Influences on youthful driving behavior and their potential for guiding interventions to reduce crashes , 2006, Injury Prevention.

[21]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  Extending the theory of planned behavior: The role of behavioral options and additional factors in predicting speed behavior , 2013 .

[22]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample , 1987, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[23]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  The effect of the commitment to observe speed limits during rehabilitation training courses for traffic regulation offenders in France , 2008 .

[24]  P. Delhomme,et al.  Quand chacun pense être moins exposé que les autres aux risques mais plus réceptif aux messages de prévention pour la santé. , 2000 .

[25]  Mark A Elliott,et al.  Evidence that changes in social cognitions predict changes in self-reported driver behavior: Causal analyses of two-wave panel data. , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[26]  M. Elliott,et al.  Drivers' compliance with speed limits: an application of the theory of planned behavior. , 2003, The Journal of applied psychology.

[27]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  Risk comparative judgments while driving a car among competitive road cyclists and non-cyclists , 2009 .

[28]  Bram P. Buunk,et al.  SOCIAL-COMPARISON OF HEALTH RISKS - LOCUS OF CONTROL, THE PERSON-POSITIVITY BIAS, AND UNREALISTIC OPTIMISM , 1993 .

[29]  Mark Stevenson,et al.  Accuracy of self-report of on-road crashes and traffic offences in a cohort of young drivers: the DRIVE study , 2010, Injury Prevention.

[30]  Mark A. Elliott,et al.  Exploring the beliefs underpinning drivers’ intentions to comply with speed limits , 2005 .

[31]  N. Konstantinou,et al.  Risky and aggressive driving in young adults: Personality matters. , 2011, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[32]  Mhairi. Campbell,et al.  Factors influencing driver speed choices , 2003 .

[33]  Marie Helweg-Larsen,et al.  Are Comparative Risk Judgments Consistent Across Time and Events? , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[34]  J. Griffet,et al.  Comparative optimism and risky road traffic behaviors among high‐risk sports practitioners , 2010 .

[35]  Mireille Chiron,et al.  Self-reported road traffic violations in France and how they have changed since 1983. , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[36]  W. Klein,et al.  Unrealistic Optimism: Present and Future , 1996 .

[37]  Dongo Rémi Kouabenan,et al.  Jugements comparatifs et absolus de deux risques routiers contexualisés et raisons invoquées quant à ces jugements , 2005 .

[38]  Richard Joiner,et al.  The typical student as an in‐group member: eliminating optimistic bias by reducing social distance , 2000 .

[39]  Lars Åberg,et al.  Speed choice in relation to speed limit and influences from other drivers , 2000 .

[40]  L. Perloff,et al.  Self–other judgments and perceived vulnerability to victimization. , 1986 .

[41]  L. Åberg,et al.  OBSERVED VEHICLE SPEED AND DRIVERS' PERCEIVED SPEED OF OTHERS , 1997 .

[42]  W. Klein,et al.  Objective standards are not enough: affective, self-evaluative, and behavioral responses to social comparison information. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  Neil D Weinstein,et al.  Taking Stock of Unrealistic Optimism , 2013, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[44]  Marco Dohle The Third-Person Effect in Communication , 2016 .

[45]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  Cyclists and drivers in road interactions: A comparison of perceived crash risk. , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[46]  Marie Helweg-Larsen,et al.  Do Moderators of the Optimistic Bias Affect Personal or Target Risk Estimates? A Review of the Literature , 2001 .

[47]  Krista W Ranby,et al.  Perceived susceptibility measures are not interchangeable: absolute, direct comparative, and indirect comparative risk. , 2010, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[48]  Darren George,et al.  SPSS for Windows Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference , 1998 .

[49]  Florence Spitzenstetter,et al.  Interdependency Between Risk Assessments for Self and Other in the Field of Comparative Optimism: The Contribution of Response Times , 2012 .

[50]  Frank P. McKenna,et al.  It won't happen to me: Unrealistic optimism or illusion of control? , 1993 .

[51]  B N Fildes,et al.  Speed behaviour and drivers' attitudes to speeding , 1989 .

[52]  R West,et al.  Direct observation of driving, self reports of driver behaviour, and accident involvement. , 1993, Ergonomics.

[53]  L D Cohn,et al.  Risk-perception: differences between adolescents and adults. , 1995, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[54]  Ingrid van Schagen,et al.  Driving speed and the risk of road crashes: a review. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[55]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[56]  James A. Shepperd,et al.  Exploring the causes of comparative optimism. , 2002 .

[57]  L Quine,et al.  Perceptions of risk in motorcyclists: unrealistic optimism, relative realism and predictions of behaviour. , 1998, British journal of psychology.

[58]  Patricia Delhomme Optimisme comparatif chez les usagers de la route : Une protection contre le risque ? , 2000 .

[59]  R. Gifford,et al.  Extending the Theory of Planned Behavior : Predicting the Use of Public Transportation ' , 2002 .

[60]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  Personality predictors of speeding in young drivers: Anger vs. sensation seeking , 2012 .