Attention and weight in person perception: The impact of negative and extreme behavior.

Social perceivers process selected aspects of the stimulus array presented by another person. This article argues that such selectivity is based on the informativeness of person attributes. Properties of the attribute itself—its evaluative extremity (distance from the scale midpoint) and its evaluative valence (positive or negative)—can make it informative. Informative attributes attract selective attention at input and also carry extra weight in the final impression. In the present research, negativity and extremity were manipulated across two separate behavioral dimensions, sociability and civic activism, and over 16 stimulus persons. Perceivers saw two prescaled behavior photographs for each stimulus person and controlled a slide changer switch—this provided a measure of attention as looking time. Perceivers also rated each stimulus person's likability, providing a measure of relative weight for each slide. Weights were derived from Anderson's information integration model. Perceivers preferentially weighted behaviors that were extreme or negative, and the behavioral measure of attention (looking time) replicated the predicted pattern. The results are discussed in light of controversies over the rationality of social information processing.

[1]  A. Tversky,et al.  On the psychology of prediction , 1973 .

[2]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[3]  Bert H. Hodges Effect of valence on relative weighting in impression formation. , 1974 .

[4]  N. Anderson Integration theory and attitude change. , 1971 .

[5]  Ebbe B. Ebbesen,et al.  Observational goals and schema activation: A theoretical framework for behavior perception , 1979 .

[6]  Susan T. Fiske,et al.  Person concepts: The effect of target familiarity and descriptive purpose on the process of describing others , 1979 .

[7]  D. K. Chalmers Repetition and order effects in attitude formation. , 1971 .

[8]  P. Jackson,et al.  The importance of extremity. , 1975 .

[9]  Norman H. Anderson,et al.  Cognitive Algebra: Integration Theory Applied to Social Attribution1 , 1974 .

[10]  P. S. Vivekananthan,et al.  A multidimensional approach to the structure of personality impressions. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  S. Asch Forming impressions of personality. , 1946, Journal of Abnormal Psychology.

[12]  Susan T. Fiske,et al.  Stigma, Staring, and Discomfort: A Novel-Stimulus Hypothesis. , 1976 .

[13]  Edward E. Jones,et al.  Person perception 2nd ed. , 1979 .

[14]  E. Berscheid,et al.  Outcome dependency: Attention, attribution, and attraction. , 1976 .

[15]  J. Haley,et al.  Self-descriptions: configurations of content and order effects. , 1974, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  N. Anderson,et al.  Combining visual and verbal information in an impression-formation task. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[18]  N. Anderson,et al.  Application of a linear-serial model to a personality-impression task using serial presentation. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  H A PODELL,et al.  QUANTITATIVE CONNOTATION OF A CONCEPT. , 1963, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[20]  E. E. Jones,et al.  From Acts To Dispositions The Attribution Process In Person Perception1 , 1965 .

[21]  Fritz Heider,et al.  Social perception and phenomenal causality. , 1944 .

[22]  G. Fontaine,et al.  Causal attribution in simulated versus real situations: When are people logical, when are they not? , 1975 .

[23]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Salience, Attention, and Attribution: Top of the Head Phenomena , 1978 .

[24]  L. Ross The Intuitive Psychologist And His Shortcomings: Distortions in the Attribution Process1 , 1977 .

[25]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research , 1977 .

[26]  E. E. Jones,et al.  The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. , 1972 .

[27]  William G. Cochran,et al.  Experimental designs, 2nd ed. , 1957 .

[28]  J. Bruner Beyond the Information Given: Studies in the Psychology of Knowing , 1973 .

[29]  N. Anderson,et al.  New light on order effects in attitude change. , 1973 .

[30]  Cognitive aspects of prejudice. , 1969 .

[31]  P. Suppes,et al.  Contemporary Developments in Mathematical Psychology , 1976 .

[32]  D. Hamilton,et al.  Generality of impression-formation processes for evaluative and nonevaluative judgments. , 1971 .

[33]  A. Parducci The relativism of absolute judgements. , 1968, Scientific American.

[34]  W. Mischel,et al.  Prototypes in Person Perception1 , 1979 .

[35]  L. Ross,et al.  The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process”, in Ed), Advances in Experimental Social New York, pp. . , 1977 .

[36]  R. Hastie Person memory : the cognitive basis of social perception , 1980 .

[37]  Susan T. Fiske,et al.  The independence of evaluative and item information: Impression and recall order effects in behavior-based impression formation. , 1979 .

[38]  J. Bruner,et al.  Compatriot and foreigner: a study of impression formation in three countries. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[39]  G. Āllport The Nature of Prejudice , 1954 .

[40]  R. Abelson Script processing in attitude formation and decision making. , 1976 .

[41]  H. Kelley Attribution theory in social psychology , 1967 .

[42]  M. Brewer Averaging versus summation in composite ratings of complex social stimuli. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  R. Malpass,et al.  Recognition for faces of own and other race. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  T. Ostrom Between-theory and within-theory conflict in explaining context effects in impression formation ☆ , 1977 .

[45]  N. Anderson,et al.  Test of a model for opinion change. , 1959, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.