Perception of exertion and attention allocation as a function of visual and auditory conditions.

Aim: The aim of the present study was to test the role of auditory (e.g., music) and visual sensory modalities on perceived levels of exertion and attention processes across a 30% dynamometer-squeezing task. The study explored the dynamics embedded within the automated nature of the association– dissociation shift process. Method: Sixty young adults (33 male and 27 female; Mage ¼ 22.21 yrs, SD ¼ 3.49) were recruited to perform a handgrip-squeezing task. They were randomly assigned to 4 groups of 15 participants. After establishing the maximal squeezing value they performed 30% max squeezing-task under one of four assigned condition: full vision and preferred music, full vision and no music, blindfolded and preferred music, and blindfolded and no music until volitional fatigue. Rate of perceived exertion and attention strategies were administered at 30 s intervals. Results: A set of Repeated Measures ANOVAs indicated an additive effect of visual and auditory cues on the perception of exertion, and the dynamic pattern of attention shift. Participants who were given both forms of sensory information remained in a dissociative strategy for a significantly longer duration than participants in all other conditions. Conclusions: Several sensory modalities are required to affect perceived exertion and attention allocation while engaging in demanding workload. External stimuli may serve as mediating agents in diverting attention away from internal and painful stimuli. This distraction may likely contribute to the pleasantness of the exercise experience, ultimately leading to increased exercise participation and reduced dropout rates. Published by Elsevier Ltd.

[1]  T Akimoto,et al.  Effects of music during exercise on RPE, heart rate and the autonomic nervous system. , 2006, The Journal of sports medicine and physical fitness.

[2]  V. Tammen Elite middle and long distance runners associative/dissociative coping , 1996 .

[3]  Gershon Tenenbaum,et al.  Attention allocation under varied workload and effort perception in rowers , 2008 .

[4]  D. Bavelier,et al.  Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  L. Hardy,et al.  Self-regulation training in sport and work. , 1988, Ergonomics.

[6]  A. Bandura Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .

[7]  G. Tenenbaum,et al.  The Effect of Manipulated Self-Efficacy on Perceived and Sustained Effort , 2008 .

[8]  L. Hyvärinen,et al.  Modification of parietal association cortex and functional blindness after binocular deprivation in young monkeys , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. , 1977, Psychological review.

[10]  E. McAuley,et al.  Manipulating self-efficacy in the exercise environment in women: influences on affective responses. , 1999, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[11]  Tim Pitt,et al.  Physiology of Sport and Exercise , 2004 .

[12]  R. Eklund,et al.  Handbook of sport psychology , 2007 .

[13]  D. Kahneman,et al.  When More Pain Is Preferred to Less: Adding a Better End , 1993 .

[14]  G. Borg Borg's Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales , 1998 .

[15]  G. Borg Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. , 1982, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[16]  R. Eklund,et al.  Handbook of sport psychology, 3rd ed. , 2007 .

[17]  E. Heath Borg's Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales , 1998 .

[18]  M. Hallett,et al.  Period of susceptibility for cross‐modal plasticity in the blind , 1999, Annals of neurology.

[19]  B. Copeland,et al.  Effects of types and intensities of background music on treadmill endurance. , 1991, The Journal of sports medicine and physical fitness.

[20]  Gershon Tenenbaum,et al.  Exertion–Attention–Flow Linkage Under Different Workloads , 2010 .

[21]  J. Hutchinson Psychological Factors in Perceived and Sustained Effort , 2004 .

[22]  M. Pollock,et al.  PSYCHOLOGIC CHARACTERIZATION OF THE ELITE DISTANCE RUNNER , 1977, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[23]  F Rösler,et al.  Different cortical activation patterns in blind and sighted humans during encoding and transformation of haptic images. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[24]  A. Bandura,et al.  Social foundation of thoughts and actions: A social cognitive theory , 1986 .

[25]  K. Masters,et al.  Associative and dissociative cognitive strategies in exercise and running: 20 years later, what do we know? , 1998 .

[26]  David Scott,et al.  The Effect of Associative and Dissociative Strategies on Rowing Ergometer Performance , 1999 .

[27]  A. Stone,et al.  Does the peak-end phenomenon observed in laboratory pain studies apply to real-world pain in rheumatoid arthritics? , 2000, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[28]  Sharon A. Plowman,et al.  Effects of Music on Exercise Performance , 1988 .

[29]  K. Koltyn,et al.  Contralateral attenuation of pain after short-duration submaximal isometric exercise. , 2007, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[30]  Gershon Tenenbaum,et al.  Perceived effort — Can it be considered gestalt? , 2006 .

[31]  Gershon Tenenbaum,et al.  THE EFFECT OF MUSIC TYPE ON RUNNING PERSEVERANCE AND COPING WITH EFFORT SENSATIONS , 2004 .

[32]  W Niemeyer,et al.  Do the blind hear better? Investigations on auditory processing in congenital or early acquired blindness. I. Peripheral functions. , 1981, Audiology : official organ of the International Society of Audiology.

[33]  C. Karageorghis,et al.  Psychophysical and ergogenic effects of synchronous music during treadmill walking. , 2009, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[34]  P. Swartz,et al.  Response to Body Rotation and Tendency to Mystical Experience , 1981 .

[35]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  A Positron Emission Tomographic Study of Auditory Localization in the Congenitally Blind , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  K B Pandolf,et al.  Influence of Local and Central Factors in Dominating Rated Perceived Exertion during Physical Work , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[37]  M. Steiner,et al.  Increased regional cerebral blood flow in inferior occipital cortex and cerebellum of early blind humans , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[38]  P. Terry,et al.  THE PSYCHOPHYSICAL EFFECTS OF MUSIC IN SPORT AND EXERCISE: A REVIEW , 1997 .

[39]  T D Noakes,et al.  Effect of anticipation during unknown or unexpected exercise duration on rating of perceived exertion, affect, and physiological function , 2005, British Journal of Sports Medicine.

[40]  W Niemeyer,et al.  Do the blind hear better? Investigations on auditory processing in congenital or early acquired blindness. II. Central functions. , 1981, Audiology : official organ of the International Society of Audiology.

[41]  C. Karageorghis,et al.  The effects of synchronous music on 400-m sprint performance , 2006, Journal of sports sciences.

[42]  Gershon Tenenbaum,et al.  Attention focus during physical effort: The mediating role of task intensity , 2007 .

[43]  Successful English Channel Swimming: The Peak Experience , 2000 .