When cognitive control is calibrated: event-related potential correlates of adapting to information-processing conflict despite erroneous response preparation.

To examine when in the perception-action cycle resolving information-processing conflict modulates signals of the current need for cognitive control, the present work examined event-related potential correlates of response preparation (lateralized readiness potentials; LRPs) and of information-processing conflict (fronto-central N2 responses) on trial n flanker trials, as a function of whether trial n-1 entailed a congruent flanker, an incongruent flanker, or a NoGo cue. Although LRP-indexed erroneous response preparation was substantial on incongruent trials across all levels of trial n-1, N2 amplitudes and behavioral interference effects were attenuated on incongruent trials following NoGo and incongruent (relative to congruent) trials. Even after initial attentional and motor-preparation processes have transpired, then, relatively later control mechanisms appear sufficient to signal a reduced need to engage cognitive control anew.

[1]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Multidimensional rule, unidimensional rule, and similarity strategies in categorization: event-related brain potential correlates. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[2]  Lauren M. Bylsma,et al.  The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[4]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[6]  S. Geisser,et al.  On methods in the analysis of profile data , 1959 .

[7]  Tobias Egner,et al.  The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task , 2005, NeuroImage.

[8]  T. Shallice,et al.  Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[10]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  R. Jong,et al.  Dissociations between motor-related EEG measures in a cued movement sequence task , 2008, Cortex.

[12]  Matthew Davidson,et al.  Context Modulates Early Stimulus Processing when Resolving Stimulus-response Conflict , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  R. McCarley,et al.  The NoGo P300 ‘anteriorization’ effect and response inhibition , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[14]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[15]  A. Fallgatter,et al.  The NoGo-anteriorization as a neurophysiological standard-index for cognitive response control. , 1999, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[16]  A. Freitas,et al.  Contextual Adjustments in Cognitive Control Across Tasks , 2007, Psychological science.

[17]  Cheryl L. Dickter,et al.  Strategic control and medial frontal negativity: beyond errors and response conflict. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[18]  E. Donchin,et al.  Preparation to respond as manifested by movement-related brain potentials , 1980, Brain Research.

[19]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Uwe Mattler,et al.  Delayed flanker effects on lateralized readiness potentials , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[23]  M. Eimer,et al.  Electrophysiological markers of visual dimension changes and response changes. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Allen Azizian,et al.  Beware misleading cues: perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[25]  David Badre,et al.  Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro–caudal organization of the frontal lobes , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  Dorret I. Boomsma,et al.  Accounting for sequential trial effects in the flanker task: Conflict adaptation or associative priming? , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[27]  Resisting recently acted-on cues: compatibility of Go/NoGo responses to response history modulates (frontal P3) event-related potentials. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[28]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Stimulus modality, perceptual overlap, and the go/no-go N2. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[29]  Monica Fabiani,et al.  Effects of alcohol consumption and alcohol susceptibility on cognition: a psychophysiological examination , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[30]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification. , 1997, Psychological review.

[31]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.

[32]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  Mike Wendt,et al.  Disentangling Sequential Effects of Stimulus- and Response-related Conflict and Stimulus-Response Repetition using Brain Potentials , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Hartmut Leuthold,et al.  Mechanisms of Priming by Masked Stimuli: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1998 .

[35]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[36]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[37]  B. Kopp,et al.  N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[38]  C. Rorden,et al.  Cognitive Control Mechanisms Revealed by ERP and fMRI: Evidence from Repeated Task-Switching , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  M. Botvinick Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  J. Fuster Upper processing stages of the perception–action cycle , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  G. Logan,et al.  Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control , 1997 .