Action-Specific Disruption of Perceptual Confidence

Theoretical models of perception assume that confidence is related to the quality or strength of sensory processing. Counter to this intuitive view, we showed in the present research that the motor system also contributes to judgments of perceptual confidence. In two experiments, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to manipulate response-specific representations in the premotor cortex, selectively disrupting postresponse confidence in visual discrimination judgments. Specifically, stimulation of the motor representation associated with the unchosen response reduced confidence in correct responses, thereby reducing metacognitive capacity without changing visual discrimination performance. Effects of TMS on confidence were observed when stimulation was applied both before and after the response occurred, which suggests that confidence depends on late-stage metacognitive processes. These results place constraints on models of perceptual confidence and metacognition by revealing that action-specific information in the premotor cortex contributes to perceptual confidence.

[1]  Hakwan Lau,et al.  Signal Detection Theory Analysis of Type 1 and Type 2 Data: Meta-d′, Response-Specific Meta-d′, and the Unequal Variance SDT Model , 2014 .

[2]  R. Passingham The frontal lobes and voluntary action , 1993 .

[3]  Ariel Zylberberg,et al.  The construction of confidence in a perceptual decision , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[4]  R. Romo,et al.  Temporal Evolution of a Decision-Making Process in Medial Premotor Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[5]  P. Matthews,et al.  The role of ipsilateral premotor cortex in hand movement after stroke , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  Wei-Hung Chen,et al.  Low-frequency rTMS over lateral premotor cortex induces lasting changes in regional activation and functional coupling of cortical motor areas , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[7]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[9]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Changing your mind: a computational mechanism of vacillation , 2009, Nature.

[10]  R. Passingham,et al.  Premotor cortex and the conditions for movement in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  Simon Barthelmé,et al.  Flexible mechanisms underlie the evaluation of visual confidence , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  P. Haggard,et al.  Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action , 2010, Cognition.

[13]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[14]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer The secret life of fluency , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Hartwig R. Siebner,et al.  BOLD MRI responses to repetitive TMS over human dorsal premotor cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[16]  David J. Freedman,et al.  A proposed common neural mechanism for categorization and perceptual decisions , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  I. Erev,et al.  Simultaneous Over- and Underconfidence: The Role of Error in Judgment Processes. , 1994 .

[18]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  H. Lau,et al.  A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating metacognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings , 2012, Consciousness and Cognition.

[20]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[21]  D. Bates,et al.  Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4 , 2015 .

[22]  R. Rafal,et al.  Localization of the human frontal eye fields and motor hand area with transcranial magnetic stimulation and magnetic resonance imaging , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  R. Dolan,et al.  The neural basis of metacognitive ability , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Confidence Associated with a Decision by Neurons in the Parietal Cortex , 2009, Science.

[25]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  The Cortical Control of Movement Revisited , 2002, Neuron.

[26]  Jacinta O'Shea,et al.  Functional specificity of human premotor–motor cortical interactions during action selection , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[27]  R. Passingham,et al.  Theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation to the prefrontal cortex impairs metacognitive visual awareness , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.

[28]  K. Mills,et al.  Corticomotor threshold to magnetic stimulation: Normal values and repeatability , 1997, Muscle & nerve.

[29]  Susan J. Galvin,et al.  Type 2 tasks in the theory of signal detectability: Discrimination between correct and incorrect decisions , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Relating Introspective Accuracy to Individual Differences in Brain Structure , 2010, Science.

[31]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Metacognition in human decision-making: confidence and error monitoring , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  R. Passingham,et al.  Temporary interference in human lateral premotor cortex suggests dominance for the selection of movements. A study using transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  Edgar C Merkle,et al.  An application of the poisson race model to confidence calibration. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  Timothy J. Pleskac,et al.  Two-stage dynamic signal detection: a theory of choice, decision time, and confidence. , 2010, Psychological review.

[36]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action , 2005, Neuron.

[37]  D. Vickers Decision processes in visual perception , 1979 .

[38]  Sanford Weisberg,et al.  An R Companion to Applied Regression , 2010 .

[39]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Cortical control of movement , 1994, Annals of neurology.

[40]  M. Kenward,et al.  An Introduction to the Bootstrap , 2007 .

[41]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of a Perceptual Decision in Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[42]  Hatim A. Zariwala,et al.  Neural correlates, computation and behavioural impact of decision confidence , 2008, Nature.

[43]  H. Steinmetz,et al.  Coil placement in magnetic brain stimulation related to skull and brain anatomy. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[44]  Tony Ro,et al.  Locating the Human Frontal Eye Fields With Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2002, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[45]  R. Dolan,et al.  Prefrontal Contributions to Metacognition in Perceptual Decision Making , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Visual receptive fields of frontal eye field neurons. , 1973, Brain research.

[48]  I. Erev,et al.  Simultaneous Over- and Underconfidence: The Role of Error in Judgment Processes. , 1994 .