The answer is blowing in the wind: free-flying honeybees can integrate visual and mechano-sensory inputs for making complex foraging decisions

ABSTRACT Bees navigate in complex environments using visual, olfactory and mechano-sensorial cues. In the lowest region of the atmosphere, the wind environment can be highly unsteady and bees employ fine motor-skills to enhance flight control. Recent work reveals sophisticated multi-modal processing of visual and olfactory channels by the bee brain to enhance foraging efficiency, but it currently remains unclear whether wind-induced mechano-sensory inputs are also integrated with visual information to facilitate decision making. Individual honeybees were trained in a linear flight arena with appetitive–aversive differential conditioning to use a context-setting cue of 3 m s−1 cross-wind direction to enable decisions about either a ‘blue’ or ‘yellow’ star stimulus being the correct alternative. Colour stimuli properties were mapped in bee-specific opponent-colour spaces to validate saliency, and to thus enable rapid reverse learning. Bees were able to integrate mechano-sensory and visual information to facilitate decisions that were significantly different to chance expectation after 35 learning trials. An independent group of bees were trained to find a single rewarding colour that was unrelated to the wind direction. In these trials, wind was not used as a context-setting cue and served only as a potential distracter in identifying the relevant rewarding visual stimuli. Comparison between respective groups shows that bees can learn to integrate visual and mechano-sensory information in a non-elemental fashion, revealing an unsuspected level of sensory processing in honeybees, and adding to the growing body of knowledge on the capacity of insect brains to use multi-modal sensory inputs in mediating foraging behaviour. Summary: In complex environments, free-flying honeybees are able to integrate both visual and wind-induced mechano-sensory cues to make decisions in a way that is suggestive of integrative processing by the brain.

[1]  Adrian G. Dyer,et al.  Flying in Complex Environments: Can Insects Bind Multiple Sensory Perceptions and What Could Be the Lessons for Machine Vision? , 2014 .

[2]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect , 2001, Nature.

[3]  M V Srinivasan,et al.  Honeybee navigation: nature and calibration of the "odometer". , 2000, Science.

[4]  B. Hansson,et al.  Interaction of visual and odour cues in the mushroom body of the hawkmoth Manduca sexta , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[5]  S. Dötterl,et al.  Visual and Olfactory Floral Cues of Campanula (Campanulaceae) and Their Significance for Host Recognition by an Oligolectic Bee Pollinator , 2015, PloS one.

[6]  David J. Anderson,et al.  Distinct sensory representations of wind and near-field sound in the Drosophila brain , 2009, Nature.

[7]  Lars Chittka,et al.  The colour hexagon: a chromaticity diagram based on photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponency , 1992, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[8]  S. Partan Ten unanswered questions in multimodal communication , 2013, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[9]  Mandyam V. Srinivasan,et al.  Vision and air flow combine to streamline flying honeybees , 2013, Scientific Reports.

[10]  Julian J. Faraway,et al.  Extending the Linear Model with R , 2004 .

[11]  Mandyam V Srinivasan,et al.  Floral scents induce recall of navigational and visual memories in honeybees , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[12]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Color modulates olfactory learning in honeybees by an occasion-setting mechanism. , 2011, Learning & memory.

[13]  Mandyam V. Srinivasan,et al.  Scent-triggered navigation in honeybees , 2004 .

[14]  R. Menzel,et al.  Chromatic properties of interneurons in the optic lobes of the bee , 2004, Journal of comparative physiology.

[15]  J. A. Stacey,et al.  Selective attention in the honeybee optic lobes precedes behavioral choices , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  Irmgard Kriston,et al.  Die Bewertung von Duft- und Farbsignalen als Orientierungshilfen an der Futterquelle durchApis mellifera L. , 1973, Journal of comparative physiology.

[17]  A. Mamiya,et al.  Neural Representations of Airflow in Drosophila Mushroom Body , 2008, PloS one.

[18]  Eileen A. Hebets,et al.  Current Status and Future Directions of Research in Complex Signaling , 2011 .

[19]  R. Murray,et al.  Flying Drosophila stabilize their vision-based velocity controller by sensing wind with their antennae , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  J. Erber,et al.  Tactile learning in the honeybee , 1998, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[21]  W. Gronenberg,et al.  Chromatic Processing in the Anterior Optic Tubercle of the Honey Bee Brain , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  R. Menzel,et al.  Detection of coloured stimuli by honeybees: minimum visual angles and receptor specific contrasts , 1996, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[23]  H. Lachnit,et al.  The effect of cumulative experience on the use of elemental and configural visual discrimination strategies in honeybees , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  J S Humbert,et al.  Kinematic strategies for mitigating gust perturbations in insects , 2013, Bioinspiration & biomimetics.

[25]  B. Kimmerle,et al.  Physiological and morphological characterization of honeybee olfactory neurons combining electrophysiology, calcium imaging and confocal microscopy , 2003, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[26]  Adrian G. Dyer,et al.  Conceptualization of relative size by honeybees , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[27]  A. Leonard,et al.  Multisensory integration of colors and scents: insights from bees and flowers , 2014, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[28]  S. W. Zhang,et al.  Honeybees link sights to smells , 1998, Nature.

[29]  Sarah E. J. Arnold,et al.  Behavioural ecology: Bees associate warmth with floral colour , 2006, Nature.

[30]  S. Combes,et al.  Rolling with the flow: bumblebees flying in unsteady wakes , 2013, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[31]  Mandyam V Srinivasan,et al.  Visual working memory in decision making by honey bees. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  Lars Chittka,et al.  Conical Epidermal Cells Allow Bees to Grip Flowers and Increase Foraging Efficiency , 2009, Current Biology.

[33]  D. A. King,et al.  Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear: tests for the associative value of the context. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[34]  Rajat Mittal,et al.  Hawkmoth flight stability in turbulent vortex streets , 2013, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[35]  M. Gewecke Antennae: Another Wind-sensitive Receptor in Locusts , 1970, Nature.

[36]  Ingolf Steffan-Dewenter,et al.  Honeybee foraging in differentially structured landscapes , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[37]  R. Bevins,et al.  Occasion setting by drug states: Functional equivalence following similar training history , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[38]  Jair E. Garcia,et al.  Color Difference and Memory Recall in Free-Flying Honeybees: Forget the Hard Problem , 2014, Insects.

[39]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  Evolution, discovery, and interpretations of arthropod mushroom bodies. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[40]  Mandyam V. Srinivasan Going with the flow: a brief history of the study of the honeybee’s navigational ‘odometer’ , 2014, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[41]  A. Kelber,et al.  The relative importance of olfaction and vision in a diurnal and a nocturnal hawkmoth , 2006, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[42]  B. Brembs,et al.  Context and occasion setting in Drosophila visual learning. , 2006, Learning & memory.

[43]  A. Dyer,et al.  A hundred years of color studies in insects: with thanks to Karl von Frisch and the workers he inspired , 2014, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[44]  H. B. Mirwan,et al.  Motion discrimination by Bombus impatiens (Hymenoptera: Apidae) , 2015, The Canadian Entomologist.

[45]  R. Menzel,et al.  Structure and response patterns of olfactory interneurons in the honeybee, Apis mellifera , 2001, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[46]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Invertebrate learning and memory: Fifty years of olfactory conditioning of the proboscis extension response in honeybees. , 2012, Learning & memory.

[47]  H. A. McCartney,et al.  Compensation for wind drift by bumble-bees , 1999, Nature.

[48]  K. Frisch The dance language and orientation of bees , 1967 .

[49]  M. Giurfa,et al.  The forest or the trees: preference for global over local image processing is reversed by prior experience in honeybees , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[50]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Behavioral Neuroscience , 2022 .

[51]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Neural substrate for higher-order learning in an insect: Mushroom bodies are necessary for configural discriminations , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[52]  M. Lane,et al.  Flower petal microtexture is a tactile cue for bees. , 1985, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  Dr. Willi A. Ribi,et al.  The Neurons of the First Optic Ganglion of the Bee (Apis mellifera) , 1975, Advances in Anatomy, Embryology and Cell Biology / Ergebnisse der Anatomie und Entwicklungsgeschichte / Revues d’anatomie et de morphologie expérimentale.

[54]  Adrian G. Dyer,et al.  Discrimination of flower colours in natural settings by the bumblebee species Bombus Terrestris (Hymenoptera: Apidae) , 2006 .

[55]  D. Dubourdieu,et al.  The Color of Odors , 2001, Brain and Language.

[56]  Werner Backhaus,et al.  Color opponent coding in the visual system of the honeybee , 1991, Vision Research.

[57]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Lifespan: Catch-up growth and obesity in male mice , 2004, Nature.

[58]  W. Gronenberg,et al.  Segregation of visual input to the mushroom bodies in the honeybee (Apis mellifera) , 2002, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[59]  R. Stull An Introduction to Boundary Layer Meteorology , 1988 .

[60]  Jair E. Garcia,et al.  Bee reverse-learning behavior and intra-colony differences: Simulations based on behavioral experiments reveal benefits of diversity , 2014 .

[61]  John B. Free,et al.  Bumblebee economics , 1979, Nature.

[62]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[63]  W. Rössler,et al.  Comparison of microglomerular structures in the mushroom body calyx of neopteran insects. , 2011, Arthropod structure & development.

[64]  Werner Backhaus,et al.  Odour and colour information in the foraging choice behaviour of the honeybee , 1994, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[65]  A. Dornhaus,et al.  Flowers help bees cope with uncertainty: signal detection and the function of floral complexity , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[66]  Z. J. Wang Nature’s Flyers: Birds, Insects, and the Biomechanics of Flight , 2007 .

[67]  J. G. Burns,et al.  Diversity of speed-accuracy strategies benefits social insects , 2008, Current Biology.

[68]  Michael B. Reiser,et al.  The role of visual and mechanosensory cues in structuring forward flight in Drosophila melanogaster , 2007, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[69]  J. Goyret Look and touch: multimodal sensory control of flower inspection movements in the nocturnal hawkmoth Manduca sexta , 2010, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[70]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Attracting the attention of a fly , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[71]  R. Menzel,et al.  The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their receptor-based colour vision , 2004, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[72]  B. Gerber,et al.  Visual modulation of olfactory learning in honeybees. , 1998, The Journal of experimental biology.

[73]  G. Amdam,et al.  Light exposure leads to reorganization of microglomeruli in the mushroom bodies and influences juvenile hormone levels in the honeybee , 2014, Developmental neurobiology.

[74]  D. Reser,et al.  Colour processing in complex environments: insights from the visual system of bees , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[75]  E. Yang,et al.  Patterns of chromatic information processing in the lobula of the honeybee, Apis mellifera L. , 2004, Journal of insect physiology.

[76]  Evgeny Osipov,et al.  Imitation of honey bees’ concept learning processes using Vector Symbolic Architectures , 2015, BICA 2015.

[77]  D. B. Judd,et al.  Spectral Distribution of Typical Daylight as a Function of Correlated Color Temperature , 1964 .

[78]  Z. J. W. Reviewer Nature’s Flyers: Birds, Insects, and the Biomechanics of Flight , 2003 .

[79]  Johannes Spaethe,et al.  Blue colour preference in honeybees distracts visual attention for learning closed shapes , 2013, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[80]  Johannes Spaethe,et al.  Visual attention in a complex search task differs between honeybees and bumblebees , 2012, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[81]  O. Helversen Das Experiment: Dressurversuche mit Bienen , 1974 .

[82]  M. Hammer,et al.  Pattern learning by honeybees: conditioning procedure and recognition strategy , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[83]  Sebastian Kirschner,et al.  Dual olfactory pathway in the honeybee, Apis mellifera , 2006, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[84]  M. Srinivasan Honey bees as a model for vision, perception, and cognition. , 2010, Annual review of entomology.

[85]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Drosophila mushroom bodies are dispensable for visual, tactile, and motor learning. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[86]  George Adrian Horridge,et al.  Shape vision in bees: innate preference for flower-like patterns , 1995 .

[87]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Aversive Reinforcement Improves Visual Discrimination Learning in Free-Flying Honeybees , 2010, PloS one.