Heuristics and Biases in Data-Based Decision Making: Effects of Experience, Training, and Graphical Data Displays

Managers use numerical data as the basis for many decisions. This research investigates how data on prior advertising expenditures and sales outcomes are used in budget allocation decisions and attempts to answer three important questions about data-based inferences. First, do biases exist that are strong enough to lead to seriously suboptimal decisions? Second, do graphical data displays, real-world experience, or explicit training reduce any observed biases? Third, are the observed biases well explained by a relatively small set of natural heuristics that managers use when making data-based allocation decisions? The results suggest answers of yes, no, and yes, respectively. The authors identify three broad classes of heuristics: difference-based (which assess causation by comparing adjacent changes in expenditures to changes in sales), trend-based (which assess causation by comparing overall trends in expenditures and sales), and exemplar-based (which emulate the allocation pattern of the observations with the highest sales). All three heuristics create biases in some situations. Overall, exemplar-based heuristics were used most frequently and induced the greatest biasing of the three (sometimes allocating the most to an advertising medium that was uncorrelated with sales). Difference-based heuristics were used less frequently but generated the most extreme allocations. Trend-based heuristics were used the least.

[1]  M. F. Luce,et al.  Debiasing Insights from Process Tests , 2006 .

[2]  Deborah Roedder John,et al.  Covariation Assessment by Consumers , 1986 .

[3]  D. Lynn Kelley,et al.  Graphing Statistics & Data: Creating Better Charts , 1996 .

[4]  Hutchinson Jw,et al.  Heuristics and biases in the "eyeballing" of data: the effects of context on intuitive correlation assessment. , 1997 .

[5]  Steven Pinker,et al.  A theory of graph comprehension. , 1990 .

[6]  G. Murphy,et al.  The utility of theories in intuitive statistics: The robustness of theory-based judgments. , 1984 .

[7]  Prabhakant Sinha,et al.  Impact of Resource Allocation Rules on Marketing Investment-Level Decisions and Profitability , 1992 .

[8]  J. W. Hutchinson,et al.  Unobserved Heterogeneity as an Alternative Explanation for 'Reversal' Effects in Behavioral Research , 2000 .

[9]  Edward R. Tufte,et al.  The Visual Display of Quantitative Information , 1986 .

[10]  F. Bolger,et al.  Graphs versus tables: Effects of data presentation format on judgemental forecasting , 1996 .

[11]  Lisa A. Best,et al.  Perception of Linear and Nonlinear Trends: Using Slope and Curvature Information to Make Trend Discriminations , 2007, Perceptual and motor skills.

[12]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  The adaptive decision maker , 1993 .

[13]  R. Nosofsky American Psychological Association, Inc. Choice, Similarity, and the Context Theory of Classification , 2022 .

[14]  J. W. Hutchinson,et al.  Knowledge Calibration: What Consumers Know and What They Think They Know , 2000 .

[15]  Michael Friendly,et al.  Visions and Re-Visions of Charles Joseph Minard , 2002 .

[16]  Sirkka L. Jarvenpaa,et al.  Graphics and managerial decision making: research-based guidelines , 1988, CACM.

[17]  Joel Huber,et al.  A theory of cutoff formation under imperfect information , 1996 .

[18]  David M. Lane,et al.  Judging the Relatedness of Variables: The Psychophysics of Covariation Detection , 1985 .

[19]  Izak Benbasat,et al.  An Experimental Evaluation of Graphical and Color-Enhanced Information Presentation , 1985 .

[20]  Joseph W. Alba,et al.  Theory versus data in prediction and correlation tasks , 1994 .

[21]  Charlotte H. Mason,et al.  Visual Representation: Implications for Decision Making , 2007 .

[22]  H. Chua,et al.  Risk avoidance: Graphs versus numbers , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[23]  L. Alloy,et al.  Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: The joint influence of prior expectations and current situational information. , 1984 .

[24]  D. Navon What does a compound letter tell the psychologist's mind? , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[25]  David K. Simkin,et al.  An Information-Processing Analysis of Graph Perception , 1987 .

[26]  Eyal Biyalogorsky,et al.  Stuck in the Past: Why Managers Persist with New Product Failures , 2006 .

[27]  Lisa E. Bolton Stickier Priors: The Effects of Nonanalytic versus Analytic Thinking in New Product Forecasting , 2003 .

[28]  Edward Rolf Tufte,et al.  The visual display of quantitative information , 1985 .

[29]  David B. Dunson,et al.  Bayesian Data Analysis , 2010 .

[30]  Sirkka L. Jarvenpaa,et al.  Graphic displays in decision making — the visual salience effect , 1990 .

[31]  J. W. Hutchinson,et al.  Action-Based Learning: Goals and Attention in the Acquisition of Market Knowledge , 2006 .

[32]  D. Krantz,et al.  The use of statistical heuristics in everyday inductive reasoning , 1983 .

[33]  Deborah F. Swayne,et al.  Interactive Graphical Methods in the Analysis of Customer Panel Data , 1996 .

[34]  Gary T. Henry,et al.  Graphing Data: Techniques for Display and Analysis , 1994 .

[35]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[36]  Paul F. Velleman,et al.  Statistical graphics: mapping the pathways of science. , 2000, Annual review of psychology.

[37]  H. Arkes Costs and benefits of judgment errors: Implications for debiasing. , 1991 .

[38]  J W Hutchinson,et al.  Heuristics and biases in the "eyeballing" of data: the effects of context on intuitive correlation assessment. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[39]  H. Simon,et al.  Models of Man. , 1957 .

[40]  John W. Tukey,et al.  Exploratory Data Analysis. , 1979 .

[41]  Jeffery. M. Zacks,et al.  Bars and lines: A study of graphic communication , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[42]  Dan Ariely,et al.  Hedonic Versus Informational Evaluations: Task Dependent Preferences for Sequences of Outcomes , 2006 .

[43]  Priti Shah,et al.  A Model of the Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Graph Comprehension , 1998 .

[44]  Chris Janiszewski,et al.  Stimulus generalization in two associative learning processes. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  R. Meyer A Theory of the Inductive Learning of Multiattribute Preferences , 1986 .

[46]  Milton D. Rosenau,et al.  Graphing statistics and data: Creating better charts: by Anders Wallgren, Britt Wallgren, Rolf Persson, Ulf Jorner, and Jan-Aage Haaland. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications, 1996. 94 pages. $45 (hard cover); $17.95 (soft cover) , 1997 .

[47]  Richard Staelin,et al.  Pulling the Plug to Stop the New Product Drain , 1997 .

[48]  Joachim Meyer,et al.  Information Structure and the Relative Efficacy of Tables and Graphs , 1999, Hum. Factors.

[49]  John G. Lynch,et al.  Smart Agents: When Lower Search Costs for Quality Information Increase Price Sensitivity , 2003 .

[50]  L. J. Chapman,et al.  Illusory correlation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic signs. , 1969, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[51]  R. Goertz,et al.  Hemispheric differences in the recognition of possible and impossible objects , 2004, Laterality.

[52]  E. Tufte Beautiful Evidence , 2006 .

[53]  P. Juslin,et al.  Exemplar effects in categorization and multiple-cue judgment. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[54]  D. Krantz,et al.  The effects of statistical training on thinking about everyday problems , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[55]  Dominique M. Hanssens,et al.  Sustained Spending and Persistent Response: A New Look at Long-Term Marketing Profitability , 1999 .

[56]  Roxann Roberson-Nay,et al.  Constructing knowledge. The role of graphs and tables in hard and soft psychology. , 2002, The American psychologist.

[57]  Howard Wainer,et al.  Visual Revelations: Graphical Tales of Fate and Deception from Napoleon Bonaparte to Ross Perot , 1997 .

[58]  Dale S. Klopfer,et al.  The perception of scatterplots , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.