Information Distortion in Physicians’ Diagnostic Judgments

Background: Information distortion suggests that people change the evaluation of new information to support an emerging belief. The present study was designed to measure the extent to which physicians distort incoming medical information to support an emerging diagnosis. Design: Data were collected via an anonymous questionnaire. The experimental group (102 physicians) read 3 patient scenarios, each with 2 competing diagnoses. Physicians first read information that favored 1 of the 2 diagnoses (the “steer”). They then rated a series of neutral cues that favored neither diagnosis. At each cue presentation, respondents rated the extent to which cues favored either diagnosis and updated the strength of their diagnostic belief. After the neutral cues in the third scenario, respondents rated cues that opposed the initial steer. A control group (36 physicians) rated all the cues in random order and not within scenarios, thus providing unbiased baseline ratings for calculating distortion in the experimental group. Results: Distortion was statistically significant (P < 0.001) and was associated with the strength of belief in the leading diagnosis. Physicians with over 10 years in practice distorted less than their less experienced counterparts ( X ¯ = 1.04 v. X ¯ = 1.78, P < 0.05). Having developed an initial diagnostic leaning consistent with the steer, 56% of physicians remained committed to it after receiving the conflicting cues. Distortion was strongly associated with commitment to the steer (odds ratio, 1.4; 95% confidence interval, 1.03–1.79; P = 0.03). Limitations: Physicians did not elicit information; therefore, the authors cannot estimate the size of distortion in tasks involving information search. Conclusions: Distortion could partly explain commitment of physicians to an early diagnosis. Both distortion and strength of initial diagnostic belief seem to decline after 10 years in family medicine.

[1]  Tadeusz Tyszka,et al.  Must boxing verdicts be biased , 1991 .

[2]  A. Brownstein,et al.  Biased predecision processing. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  J. E. Russo,et al.  The distortion of information to support an emerging evaluation of risk , 2011 .

[4]  Ola Svenson,et al.  Emerging Perspectives on Judgment and Decision Research: Values, Affect, and Processes in Human Decision Making: A Differentiation and Consolidation Theory Perspective , 2003 .

[5]  J. Edward Russo,et al.  Predecisional Distortion of Information by Auditors and Salespersons , 2000 .

[6]  T. Wallsten Physician and Medical Student Bias in Evaluating Diagnostic Information , 1981, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[7]  J. Hershey,et al.  Value-Induced Bias in Medical Decision Making , 2008, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[8]  L. Shulman,et al.  Medical Problem Solving: An Analysis of Clinical Reasoning , 1978 .

[9]  O. Kostopoulou,et al.  Information search and information distortion in the diagnosis of an ambiguous presentation , 2009, Judgment and Decision Making.

[10]  C. Harries,et al.  Predictors of Diagnostic Accuracy and Safe Management in Difficult Diagnostic Problems in Family Medicine , 2008, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[11]  Margaret G. Meloy,et al.  Predecisional Distortion of Product Information , 1998 .

[12]  Margaret G. Meloy,et al.  Choosing an Inferior Alternative , 2006, Psychological science.

[13]  Margaret G. Meloy,et al.  The goal of consistency as a cause of information distortion. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  D. Medin,et al.  Decision making from a cognitive perspective , 1995 .

[15]  O. Kostopoulou,et al.  Missing Celiac Disease in Family Medicine: The Importance of Hypothesis Generation , 2009, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[16]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  S. Read,et al.  Bias at the Racetrack: Effects of Individual Expertise and Task Importance on Predecision Reevaluation of Alternatives , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[18]  H. Simon,et al.  Skill in Chess , 1988 .

[19]  S. Read,et al.  The redux of cognitive consistency theories: evidence judgments by constraint satisfaction. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Deliberate practice and the acquisition and maintenance of expert performance in medicine and related domains. , 2004, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[21]  J. Klayman Varieties of Confirmation Bias , 1995 .

[22]  V R Neufeld,et al.  The clinical reasoning of randomly selected physicians in general medical practice. , 1982, Clinical and investigative medicine. Medecine clinique et experimentale.

[23]  J. E. Russo,et al.  Biased interpretation of evidence by mock jurors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[24]  Matthew Sibbald,et al.  The biasing effect of clinical history on physical examination diagnostic accuracy , 2011, Medical education.

[25]  Eric R. Stone,et al.  Leader‐driven distortion of probability and payoff information affects choices between risky prospects , 2011 .

[26]  M. Dekay,et al.  Distortion of Probability and Outcome Information in Risky Decisions. , 2009 .

[27]  C. Dubeau,et al.  Premature Conclusions in the Diagnosis of Iron-deficiency Anemia , 1986, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[28]  R. Hogarth,et al.  Order effects in belief updating: The belief-adjustment model , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.