Spatial and temporal patterns of sound production in East Greenland narwhals

Changes in climate are rapidly modifying the Arctic environment. As a result, human activities—and the sounds they produce—are predicted to increase in remote areas of Greenland, such as those inhabited by the narwhals (Monodon monoceros) of East Greenland. Meanwhile, nothing is known about these whales’ acoustic behavior or their reactions to anthropogenic sounds. This lack of knowledge was addressed by instrumenting six narwhals in Scoresby Sound (Aug 2013–2016) with Acousonde™ acoustic tags and satellite tags. Continuous recordings over up to seven days were used to describe the acoustic behavior of the whales, in particular their use of three types of sounds serving two different purposes: echolocation clicks and buzzes, which serve feeding, and calls, presumably used for social communication. Logistic regression models were used to assess the effects of location in time and space on buzzing and calling rates. Buzzes were mostly produced at depths of 350–650 m and buzzing rates were higher in one particular fjord, likely a preferred feeding area. Calls generally occurred at shallower depths (<100 m), with more than half of these calls occurring near the surface (<7 m), where the whales also spent more than half of their time. A period of silence following release, present in all subjects, was attributed to the capture and tagging operations, emphasizing the importance of longer (multi-day) records. This study provides basic life-history information on a poorly known species—and therefore control data in ongoing or future sound-effect studies.

[1]  R. W. Baird,et al.  Repeated call types in Hawaiian melon-headed whales (Peponocephala electra). , 2014, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  Christian Lydersen,et al.  Distribution of endemic cetaceans in relation to hydrocarbon development and commercial shipping in a warming Arctic , 2014 .

[3]  William A. Watkins,et al.  Underwater Sounds of Monodon (Narwhal) , 1971 .

[4]  Mark P. Johnson,et al.  Studying the behaviour and sensory ecology of marine mammals using acoustic recording tags: a review , 2009 .

[5]  R. Dietz,et al.  Autumn movements, home ranges, and winter density of narwhals (Monodon monoceros) tagged in Tremblay Sound, Baffin Island , 2002, Polar Biology.

[6]  Mark P. Johnson,et al.  DOES INTENSE SHIP NOISE DISRUPT FORAGING IN DEEP-DIVING CUVIER'S BEAKED WHALES (ZIPHIUS CAVIROSTRIS)? , 2006 .

[7]  Randall S. Wells,et al.  Signature whistles of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus: stability and mother-offspring comparisons , 1990, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[8]  S. Ferguson,et al.  Differences in dive behaviour among the world's three narwhal Monodon monoceros populations correspond with dietary differences , 2015 .

[9]  J. Teilmann,et al.  Long-term tag retention on two species of small cetaceans , 2017 .

[10]  G. Raposo,et al.  BLOC-1 Brings Together the Actin and Microtubule Cytoskeletons to Generate Recycling Endosomes , 2016, Current Biology.

[11]  S. Ferguson,et al.  How adaptable are narwhal? A comparison of foraging patterns among the world's three narwhal populations , 2013 .

[12]  L. Miller,et al.  THE CLICK‐SOUNDS OF NARWHALS (MONODON MONOCEROS) IN INGLEFIELD BAY, NORTHWEST GREENLAND , 1995 .

[13]  M. Humphries,et al.  Variability and context specificity of narwhal (Monodon monoceros) whistles and pulsed calls , 2012 .

[14]  Walter M. X. Zimmer,et al.  Beaked whales echolocate on prey , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[16]  Mark P. Johnson,et al.  Sperm whale behaviour indicates the use of echolocation click buzzes ‘creaks’ in prey capture , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  K. Laidre,et al.  First acoustic recordings of narwhals (Monodon monoceros) in winter , 2012 .

[18]  L. Miller,et al.  High Intensity Narwhal Clicks , 1990 .

[19]  K. Laidre,et al.  Highly Directional Sonar Beam of Narwhals (Monodon monoceros) Measured with a Vertical 16 Hydrophone Array , 2016, PloS one.

[20]  Mark P. Johnson,et al.  Following a Foraging Fish-Finder: Diel Habitat Use of Blainville's Beaked Whales Revealed by Echolocation , 2011, PloS one.

[21]  Mark P. Johnson,et al.  Foraging Blainville's beaked whales (Mesoplodon densirostris) produce distinct click types matched to different phases of echolocation , 2006, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[22]  Nathan D. Merchant,et al.  Evidence for ship noise impacts on humpback whale foraging behaviour , 2016, Biology Letters.

[23]  R. Dietz,et al.  Upside-down swimming behaviour of free-ranging narwhals , 2007, BMC Ecology.

[24]  Mads Peter Heide-Jørgensen,et al.  The predictable narwhal: satellite tracking shows behavioural similarities between isolated subpopulations , 2015 .

[25]  S. B. Blackwell,et al.  Paradoxical escape responses by narwhals (Monodon monoceros) , 2017, Science.

[26]  K. Laidre,et al.  Buzzes and high-frequency clicks recorded from narwhals (Monodon monoceros) at their wintering ground , 2015 .

[27]  R. Dietz,et al.  Population structure and seasonal movements of narwhals, Monodon monoceros, determined from mtDNA analysis , 1997, Heredity.

[28]  A. Shapirob Preliminary evidence for signature vocalizations among free-ranging narwhals ( Monodon monoceros ) a ) , 2006 .

[29]  R. Dietz,et al.  Use of glacial fronts by narwhals (Monodon monoceros) in West Greenland , 2015, Biology Letters.

[30]  Mark P. Johnson,et al.  Echolocation clicks of free-ranging Cuvier's beaked whales (Ziphius cavirostris). , 2005, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  J. Teilmann,et al.  Ultra-High Foraging Rates of Harbor Porpoises Make Them Vulnerable to Anthropogenic Disturbance , 2016, Current Biology.

[32]  P. Slater,et al.  Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[33]  Mark P. Johnson,et al.  On-Animal Methods for Studying Echolocation in Free-Ranging Animals , 2014 .

[34]  Michael D. Scott,et al.  Individual recognition in wild bottlenose dolphins: a field test using playback experiments , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[35]  P. Madsen,et al.  Buzzing during biosonar-based interception of prey in the delphinids Tursiops truncatus and Pseudorca crassidens , 2014, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[36]  P. Arranz,et al.  Echolocation in Blainville’s beaked whales (Mesoplodon densirostris) , 2013, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[37]  R. Reeves,et al.  Narwhals and seismic exploration: Is seismic noise increasing the risk of ice entrapments? , 2013 .

[38]  John K. B. Ford,et al.  Underwater acoustic signals of the narwhal (Monodon monoceros) , 1978 .

[39]  E. Kalko,et al.  Sonar Signals of Bats and Toothed Whales , 2014 .

[40]  Denise L. Herzing,et al.  Synchronous and Rhythmic Vocalizations and Correlated Underwater Behavior of Free-ranging Atlantic Spotted Dolphins (Stenella frontalis) and Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Bahamas , 2015 .

[41]  Susanna B Blackwell,et al.  Stomach temperature of narwhals (Monodon monoceros) during feeding events , 2014, Animal Biotelemetry.

[42]  Alexander Bahr,et al.  Acoustic behaviour of echolocating porpoises during prey capture , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[43]  H. Melling,et al.  Comparing marine mammal acoustic habitats in Atlantic and Pacific sectors of the High Arctic: year-long records from Fram Strait and the Chukchi Plateau , 2012, Polar Biology.