Diminished motor skill development in elderly: indications for limited motor chunk use.

The present study examined whether elderly use motor chunks after practicing discrete keying sequences, just like young adults, or whether they perhaps learn these movement patterns in a different way. To that end, elderly (75-88) and young adults (18-28) practiced as part of the discrete sequence production (DSP) task two fixed series of three and six key presses. The results demonstrate that elderly did improve with practice but this improvement was largely sequence-unspecific. Detailed analyses showed that, in contrast to young adults, most elderly did not use motor chunks, had little explicit sequence knowledge, and remained highly dependent on external stimuli. Still, elderly did show sequence-specific learning with a 6-key sequence that can be explained by an associative learning mechanism.

[1]  Willem B. Verwey,et al.  Evidence for a multistage model of practice in a sequential movement task. , 1999 .

[2]  C. Barnett,et al.  Effects of age upon the mobility of human finger joints. , 1968, Annals of the rheumatic diseases.

[3]  Charles H Shea,et al.  Age-related effects in sequential motor learning. , 2006, Physical therapy.

[4]  G. Stelmach Information processing in motor control and learning , 1978 .

[5]  W. Verwey BUFFER LOADING AND CHUNKING IN SEQUENTIAL KEYPRESSING , 1996 .

[6]  V. Dietz,et al.  The influence of age on learning a locomotor task , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[7]  R. Dixon,et al.  Age-related cognitive deficits mediated by changes in the striatal dopamine system. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[8]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  White matter tracts associated with set-shifting in healthy aging , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  Learning and production of movement sequences: behavioral, neurophysiological, and modeling perspectives. , 2004, Human movement science.

[10]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Mechanisms of Implicit Learning: Connectionist Models of Sequence Processing , 1993 .

[11]  K. Doya,et al.  Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[12]  Charles H Shea,et al.  Sequence Learning: Response Structure and Effector Transfer , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  Tim Curran,et al.  Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: Accounting for sequence structure and explicit knowledge , 1997, Psychological research.

[14]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  The Latency and Duration of Rapid Movement Sequences: Comparisons of Speech and Typewriting , 1978 .

[15]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[16]  S. T. Klapp,et al.  Motor response programming during simple choice reaction time: The role of practice. , 1995 .

[17]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  G. Band,et al.  Explaining Neurocognitive Aging: Is One Factor Enough? , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  P. Rabbitt The Alan Welford Memorial Lecture. Ageing and human skill: a 40th anniversary. , 1997, Ergonomics.

[20]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  R. Seidler,et al.  Age-related declines in visuospatial working memory correlate with deficits in explicit motor sequence learning. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  L. Nilsson,et al.  Ageing, cognition, and neuroscience , 2009 .

[23]  C. Shea,et al.  Principles derived from the study of simple skills do not generalize to complex skill learning , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[24]  W. Prinz,et al.  Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination , 2001, Nature.

[25]  Jack van Honk,et al.  On the role of the SMA in the discrete sequence production task: a TMS study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  Charles H Shea,et al.  Part-Whole Practice of Movement Sequences , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[27]  B. Cratty Movement behavior and motor learning , 1967 .

[28]  Ben Godde,et al.  Physical and motor fitness are both related to cognition in old age , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  Gavan Lintern,et al.  Part-Task Training for Tracking and Manual Control , 1985 .

[30]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Age-related changes in memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. , 2000 .

[31]  Christopher Hertzog,et al.  Memory Self-Knowledge and Self-Efficacy in the Aged , 1988 .

[32]  Robert Gottsdanker,et al.  Studying reaction time with nonaging intervals: An effective procedure , 1986 .

[33]  R. Emmerson,et al.  Physical Activity, Age, and Cognitive- Neuropsychological Function , 1994 .

[34]  M. Naveh-Benjamin Adult age differences in memory performance: tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  W B Verwey,et al.  Concatenating familiar movement sequences: the versatile cognitive processor. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[36]  N. Raz,et al.  Aging white matter and cognition: Differential effects of regional variations in diffusion properties on memory, executive functions, and speed , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  C. Vaidya,et al.  Implicit spatial contextual learning in healthy aging. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[38]  William B Verwey,et al.  Evidence for Lasting Sequence Segmentation in the Discrete Sequence-Production Task , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[39]  D. G. MacKay The Problems of Flexibility, Fluency, and Speed-Accuracy Trade-Off in Skilled Behavior. , 1982 .

[40]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Chunking during human visuomotor sequence learning , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[42]  R. Cabeza Cognitive neuroscience of aging: contributions of functional neuroimaging. , 2001, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[43]  R. Seidler,et al.  Visuospatial working memory capacity predicts the organization of acquired explicit motor sequences. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Rainer Spiegel,et al.  Associative sequence learning in humans. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[45]  Jane E. Clark,et al.  Age-related changes in multi-finger interactions in adults during maximum voluntary finger force production tasks. , 2008, Human movement science.

[46]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Effects of Age and Practice in Sequence Learning a Graded Account of Aging, Learning and Control , 2008 .

[47]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  Adult age differences in goal activation and goal maintenance , 2001 .

[48]  James H. Howard,et al.  Implicit sequence learning without motor sequencing in young and old adults , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  R. Elble Tremor in ostensibly normal elderly people , 1998, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[50]  W. E. Hick Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.

[51]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Organization of action sequences and the role of the pre-SMA. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  J. Yesavage,et al.  Context processing in older adults: evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[53]  O. Delbono Neural control of aging skeletal muscle , 2003, Aging cell.

[54]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Aging and reflective processes of working memory: binding and test load deficits. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[55]  L. Nilsson,et al.  Ageing, cognition, and neuroscience: An introduction , 2009 .

[56]  Pamela K. Smith,et al.  Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[57]  Luis Jiménez,et al.  Taking patterns for chunks: is there any evidence of chunk learning in continuous serial reaction-time tasks? , 2008, Psychological research.

[58]  Willem B Verwey,et al.  Processing modes and parallel processors in producing familiar keying sequences , 2003, Psychological research.

[59]  Claudia Voelcker-Rehage,et al.  Motor-skill learning in older adults—a review of studies on age-related differences , 2008, European Review of Aging and Physical Activity.

[60]  Luis Jiménez,et al.  Segmentation of short keying sequences does not spontaneously transfer to other sequences. , 2009, Human movement science.