Effect of driving experience on visual behavior and driving performance under different driving conditions

Drivers’ ability to perform the primary task of driving a vehicle and be able to handle the interference caused by the secondary tasks such as the use of cell phone is greatly influenced by their driving experience. In this study, thirty-eight drivers with different levels of driving experience were studied to evaluate their driving performance during simple versus complex driving tasks performed with and without cell phone. The driving performance was evaluated by studying the visual behavior of the drivers using eye-tracking systems, driving error, and subjective workload assessment tool. Results demonstrate that the novice drivers fixated more on the dashboard area (36.33%) than on the front and center view (14.33%). On the other hand, the experienced drivers fixated more on the front and center (39.5%) as compared to the dashboard area (12%). Same trend was observed for complex driving but with lower percentage of total fixations than the simple driving conditions. The visual distraction caused by secondary task of cell phone use contributes to the decline in driving performance. Although all drivers reported higher workloads during driving while using cell phones and committed more driving errors, the errors were comparatively lower for experienced drivers than the inexperienced drivers.

[1]  David Crundall,et al.  Effects of experience and processing demands on visual information acquisition in drivers , 1998 .

[2]  Louis Tijerina,et al.  Driver Workload Effects of Cell Phone, Music Player, and Text Messaging Tasks with the Ford SYNC Voice Interface versus Handheld Visual-Manual Interfaces , 2009 .

[3]  Kristen E Beede,et al.  Engrossed in conversation: the impact of cell phones on simulated driving performance. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[4]  Peter A. Hancock,et al.  The distraction effects of phone use during a crucial driving maneuver. , 2003 .

[5]  A J McKnight,et al.  The effect of cellular phone use upon driver attention. , 1993, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[6]  David Shinar,et al.  Effects of practice, age, and task demands, on interference from a phone task while driving. , 2005, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[7]  Georg Jahn,et al.  Peripheral detection as a workload measure in driving: Effects of traffic complexity and route guidance system use in a driving study , 2005 .

[8]  D. C. Donderi,et al.  The effects of cellular phone use on novice and experienced driver performance: An on-road study: (578302012-018) , 2008 .

[9]  Miwa Hayashi,et al.  Hidden Markov model analysis for space shuttle crewmembers' scanning behavior , 2005, 2005 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics.

[10]  D. C. Donderi,et al.  The effects of cellphone and CD use on novice and experienced driver performance , 2007 .

[11]  Mary Pat McKay,et al.  National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) notes. Children injured in motor vehicle traffic crashes. Commentary. , 2010, Annals of emergency medicine.

[12]  Kristin S. Moore,et al.  Mobile phone use in a driving simulation task: Differences in eye movements , 2010 .

[13]  Albert Kircher,et al.  Driver experience and cognitive workload in different traffic environments. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[14]  Kazunori Shidoji,et al.  The cause of traffic accidents when drivers use car phones and the functional requirements of car phones for safe driving , 2001 .

[15]  Karel Brookhuis,et al.  The feasibility of detecting phone-use related driver distraction , 2001 .

[16]  Jens Rasmussen,et al.  Cognitive Control and Human Error Mechanisms , 1987 .

[17]  Matthew Witte,et al.  Effect of cellular telephone conversations and other potential interference on reaction time in a braking response. , 2003, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[18]  Eric Rodgman,et al.  The role of driver distraction in traffic crashes , 2001 .

[19]  Bryan Reimer,et al.  On-road driver eye movement tracking using head-mounted devices , 2002, ETRA.

[20]  R. Tibshirani,et al.  Association between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. , 1997, The New England journal of medicine.

[21]  G. Underwood,et al.  Driving Experience and the Functional Field of View , 1999, Perception.

[22]  S. Hart,et al.  Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of Empirical and Theoretical Research , 1988 .

[23]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Driven to Distraction: Dual-Task Studies of Simulated Driving and Conversing on a Cellular Telephone , 2001, Psychological science.

[24]  M. Woodward,et al.  Systematic review and meta-analysis of strategies for the diagnosis of suspected pulmonary embolism , 2005, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[25]  C. Collet,et al.  Physiological and behavioural changes associated to the management of secondary tasks while driving. , 2009, Applied ergonomics.

[26]  D. Strayer,et al.  Cell phone-induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[27]  Michael J. Goodman,et al.  The role of driver inattention in crashes: new statistics from the 1995 crashworthiness data system , 1996 .

[28]  Andrew Liu Chapter 20 – What the Driver's Eye Tells the Car's Brain , 1998 .

[29]  Claudia J. Stanny,et al.  Effects of distraction and experience on situation awareness and simulated driving , 2007 .

[30]  Kerrie L Schattler,et al.  Assessing driver distraction from cell phone use: A simulator-based study , 2006 .

[31]  M. Pickering,et al.  Eye guidance in reading and scene perception , 1998 .

[32]  Jeff K. Caird,et al.  Effects of Passenger and Cellular Phone Conversations on Driver Distraction , 2004 .

[33]  Johnell Brooks,et al.  Effects of remote and in-person verbal interactions on verbalization rates and attention to dynamic spatial scenes. , 2004, Accident Analysis and Prevention.

[34]  Normand Teasdale,et al.  Mental workload when driving in a simulator: effects of age and driving complexity. , 2009, Accident; analysis and prevention.