The impact of sleep duration and subject intelligence on declarative and motor memory performance: how much is enough?

Recent findings clearly demonstrate that daytime naps impart substantial memory benefits compared with equivalent periods of wakefulness. Using a declarative paired associates task and a procedural motor sequence task, this study examined the effect of two lengthier durations of nocturnal sleep [either a half night (3.5 h) or a full night (7.5 h) of sleep] on over‐sleep changes in memory performance. We also assessed whether subject intelligence is associated with heightened task acquisition and, more importantly, whether greater intelligence translates to greater over‐sleep declarative and procedural memory enhancement. Across both tasks, we demonstrate that postsleep performance gains are nearly equivalent, regardless of whether subjects obtain a half night or a full night of sleep. Remarkably, the over‐sleep memory changes observed on both tasks are very similar to findings from studies examining performance following a daytime nap. Consistent with previous research, we also observed a strong positive correlation between amount of Stage 2 sleep and motor skill performance in the full‐night sleep group. This finding contrasts with a highly significant correlation between spectral power in the spindle frequency band (12–15 Hz) and motor skill enhancement only in the half‐night group, suggesting that sigma power and amount of Stage 2 sleep are both important for optimal motor memory processing. While subject intelligence correlated positively with acquisition and retest performance on both tasks, it did not correlate with over‐sleep changes in performance on either task, suggesting that intelligence may not be a powerful modulator of sleep’s effect on memory performance.

[1]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Sleep spindle‐related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[2]  G. Lawton Why do we sleep? , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  C. Degueldre,et al.  Are Spatial Memories Strengthened in the Human Hippocampus during Slow Wave Sleep? , 2004, Neuron.

[4]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Declarative memory consolidation in humans: a prospective functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  J. Hobson,et al.  Visual Discrimination Task Improvement: A Multi-Step Process Occurring During Sleep , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Born,et al.  Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[7]  J. Born,et al.  Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  J. Hobson,et al.  Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  J. Born,et al.  Low acetylcholine during slow-wave sleep is critical for declarative memory consolidation , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  A. Karni,et al.  Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. , 1994, Science.

[11]  Carlyle T. Smith,et al.  Posttraining increases in REM sleep intensity implicate REM sleep in memory processing and provide a biological marker of learning potential. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[12]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Interactive reportWhy do we sleep?1 , 2000 .

[13]  Carlyle T. Smith,et al.  Impaired motor memory for a pursuit rotor task following Stage 2 sleep loss in college students , 1994, Journal of sleep research.

[14]  E. Wamsley,et al.  A daytime nap containing solely non-REM sleep enhances declarative but not procedural memory , 2006, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[15]  Jessica D. Payne,et al.  The role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none? , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Sleep-dependent learning: a nap is as good as a night , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Johns,et al.  A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. , 1991, Sleep.

[18]  Olaf Lahl,et al.  An ultra short episode of sleep is sufficient to promote declarative memory performance , 2008, Journal of sleep research.

[19]  C. Smith,et al.  Increases in number of REMS and REM density in humans following an intensive learning period. , 1991, Sleep.

[20]  Stuart M Fogel,et al.  Learning‐dependent changes in sleep spindles and Stage 2 sleep , 2006, Journal of sleep research.

[21]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep and the time course of motor skill learning. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[22]  M. Tucker,et al.  Enhancement of declarative memory performance following a daytime nap is contingent on strength of initial task acquisition. , 2008, Sleep.

[23]  M. Walker,et al.  Daytime Naps, Motor Memory Consolidation and Regionally Specific Sleep Spindles , 2007, PloS one.

[24]  Kevin R. Peters,et al.  Changes in Sleep Architecture following Motor Learning Depend on Initial Skill Level , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Jeffrey K. Smith,et al.  The Consequence of Consequence: Motivation, Anxiety, and Test Performance , 1995 .

[26]  J. Born,et al.  Effects of Early and Late Nocturnal Sleep on Declarative and Procedural Memory , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Practice with Sleep Makes Perfect Sleep-Dependent Motor Skill Learning , 2002, Neuron.

[28]  J. Wixted CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE A Theory About Why We Forget What We Once Knew , 2022 .

[29]  K. Junghanns,et al.  Daytime naps improve procedural motor memory. , 2006, Sleep medicine.