Visual-spatial orienting in autism

Visual-spatial orienting in high-functioning adults with autism and both chronological- and mental-age normal controls was examined. Three experiments were conducted in which stimuli were presented centrally and/or laterally (left or right of central fixation), and either detection or identification was required. The group with autism differed from normal controls by responding faster to central than to lateral stimuli, and by showing a left visual field advantage for stimulus detection only in the simplest condition (lateral presentations alone). Discussion focuses on the apparent abnormalities in disengaging/shifting attention, and on the coordination of attentional and motor systems in autism.

[1]  R. Hobson Beyond cognition: A theory of autism. , 1989 .

[2]  A. Milner,et al.  Interference effects of verbal and spatial tasks on simple visual reaction time , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  B. Pennington,et al.  Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. , 1991, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[4]  L. Schreibman,et al.  Selective responding by autistic children to multiple sensory input. , 1971, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[5]  Eric Courchesne,et al.  Parietal Damage and Narrow Spotlight Spatial Attention , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Burack,et al.  Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: preliminary evidence of an inefficient attentional lens. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[7]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The question of left hemisphere dysfunction in infantile autism. , 1984, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  B. Pennington,et al.  Asperger's syndrome: evidence of an empirical distinction from high-functioning autism. , 1991, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[9]  P. Mundy,et al.  Defining the social deficits of autism: the contribution of non-verbal communication measures. , 1986, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[10]  E. Titchener V.—SIMPLE REACTIONS , 1895 .

[11]  Susan E. Bryson,et al.  Visual orienting deficits in high-functioning people with autism , 1993, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[12]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Dysfunctional attention in autistic savants. , 1993, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[13]  P. Mundy,et al.  A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children , 1990, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[14]  K. Loveland,et al.  Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay , 1986, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[15]  G. Dawson Autism : nature, diagnosis, and treatment , 1989 .

[16]  P. Mundy,et al.  The theoretical implications of joint-attention deficits in autism , 1989, Development and Psychopathology.

[17]  J. Ducharme,et al.  Variables influencing stimulus overselectivity and "tunnel vision" in developmentally delayed children. , 1987, American journal of mental deficiency.

[18]  T. Bashore,et al.  Vocal and manual reaction time estimates of interhemispheric transmission time. , 1981, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  C. A. Marzi,et al.  Distribution in the visual field of the costs of voluntarily allocated attention and of the inhibitory after-effects of covert orienting , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders : implications for brain function , 1994 .

[21]  James T. Enns,et al.  Attention, Development, and Psychopathology , 1997 .

[22]  Geraldine Dawson,et al.  Attention and the socioemotional impairments of individuals with autism , 1989 .

[23]  R. Michels,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd ed , 1981 .

[24]  Gillian Cohen,et al.  Hemispheric differences in a letter classification task , 1972 .

[25]  Mm. Liabre Standard Progressive Matrices , 1984 .

[26]  M. Drake Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed. rev.) , 1988 .

[27]  S. Bryson,et al.  Imitation and action in autism: a critical review. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  Simple reaction times of ipsilateral and contralateral hand to lateralized visual stimuli. , 1971, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  J. Rumsey Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men , 1985, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[30]  G. Hynd,et al.  The development of functional lateralization in visual hemifield attention , 1985 .

[31]  M. Kinsbourne Cerebral-Brainstem Relations in Infantile Autism , 1987 .

[32]  S. Bryson,et al.  Chapter 21 Autism: A Developmental Spatial Neglect Syndrome? , 1990 .

[33]  M. Posner,et al.  Chapter 3 Regulatory Mechanisms in Infant Development , 1990 .

[34]  Maureen Dennis,et al.  Clinical Neuropsychology and Brain Function: Research, Measurement, and Practice , 1988 .

[35]  K. Heilman,et al.  Right hemispheric dominance for mediating cerebral activation , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  G. Kanjilal Psychological Experiments with Autistic Children , 1970, Mental Health.

[37]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Structures and Functions of Selective Attention. , 1987 .

[38]  N. O’connor,et al.  Perceptual and motor discrimination in psychotic and normal children. , 1967, The Journal of genetic psychology.