Information-based and experience-based metacognitive judgments: Evidence from subjective confidence.

Dual-process theories have been very influential in social psychology and cognitive psychology. These theories postulate a distinction between two modes of thought that underlie judgment and behavior (see Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Kahneman & Frederick, 2005). Different labels have been proposed to describe the two modes (see Koriat, Bjork, Sheffer, & Bar, 2004): nonanalytic versus analytic (Jacoby & Brooks, 1984), associative versus rule based (Sloman, 1996), impulsive versus reflective (Strack & Deutsch, 2004), experiential versus rational (Epstein & Pacini, J999), and heuristic versus systematic (Chaiken, Liberman, & Eagly, 1989; Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993). Although each of these labels emphasizes different aspects of the distinction, there is a general agreement that one mode of thought is fast, automatic, effortless, and implicit, whereas the other is slow, deliberate, effortful, and consciously monitored. Several researchers preferred to use the labels proposed by Stanovich and West (2000), System 1 versus System 2, which are more neutral. A similar dual-process framework has been proposed for the analysis of metacognitive monitoring, focusing on the qnestion of how people know that they know. 11,e distinction is between experience-based (EB) and information-based (IB) metacognitive judgments (Koriat, 2007; Koriat & Levy-Sadot, 1999; Strack, 1992). 11,econceptualization of this distinction brings to the fore specific features that may have some bearing for dual-process views in general. In the rest of the introduction, we first describe this distinction and then illustrate how it was applied in research on judgments of learning (JOLs) and feelings of knowing (FOKs). In the experimental parl of the chapter, we show how reliance on experience-driven and information-driven processes can yield diametrically opposed effects.

[1]  Asher Koriat,et al.  The intricate relationships between monitoring and control in metacognition: lessons for the cause-and-effect relation between subjective experience and behavior. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  A. Tversky,et al.  The weighing of evidence and the determinants of confidence , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  G. Clore,et al.  Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. , 1983 .

[4]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Recollection and familiarity , 2000 .

[5]  A. Koriat,et al.  Processes underlying metacognitive judgments: Information-based and experience-based monitoring of one's own knowledge. , 1999 .

[6]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Illusions of competence during study can be remedied by manipulations that enhance learners’ sensitivity to retrieval conditions at test , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[7]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Predicting one's own forgetting: the role of experience-based and theory-based processes. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  The "Tip of the Tongue" Phenomenon , 1966 .

[9]  Asher Koriat,et al.  The effects of encoding fluency and retrieval fluency on judgments of learning , 2005 .

[10]  Alice H. Eagly,et al.  Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. , 1989 .

[11]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  When People's Judgments of Learning (JOLs) are Extremely Accurate at Predicting Subsequent Recall: The “Delayed-JOL Effect” , 1991 .

[12]  Seymour Epstein,et al.  Some basic issues regarding dual-process theories from the perspective of cognitive–experiential self-theory. , 1999 .

[13]  Christian Unkelbach,et al.  The Learned Interpretation of Cognitive Fluency , 2006, Psychological science.

[14]  N. Schwarz,et al.  How Pleasant Was Your Childhood? Beliefs About Memory Shape Inferences From Experienced Difficulty of Recall , 2001, Psychological science.

[15]  Craig R. M. McKenzie,et al.  Underweighting Alternatives and Overconfidence , 1997 .

[16]  M. Usher,et al.  Dynamics of metacognitive judgments: pre- and postretrieval mechanisms. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  C. Klin,et al.  Knowing that you don't know: metamemory and discourse processing. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  M D Robinson,et al.  Reaction time and assessments of cognitive effort as predictors of eyewitness memory accuracy and confidence. , 1997, The Journal of applied psychology.

[19]  G. Haddock It's easy to like or dislike Tony Blair: accessibility experiences and the favourability of attitude judgments. , 2002, British journal of psychology.

[20]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Adult Egocentrism: Subjective Experience versus Analytic Bases for Judgment , 1996 .

[21]  Lynne M. Reder,et al.  Strategic control of retrieval strategies. , 1988 .

[22]  R. Petty,et al.  Ease of Retrieval Effects in Persuasion: A Self-Validation Analysis , 2002 .

[23]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Easy comes, easy goes? The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[24]  A Koriat,et al.  The combined contributions of the cue-familiarity and accessibility heuristics to feelings of knowing. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? , 2002 .

[26]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Mending metacognitive illusions: a comparison of mnemonic-based and theory-based procedures. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  A Koriat,et al.  How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. , 1993, Psychological review.

[28]  R. Bjork,et al.  Illusions of competence in monitoring one's knowledge during study. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  J. Metcalfe Cognitive Optimism: Self-Deception or Memory-Based Processing Heuristics? , 1998, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[30]  John Dunlosky,et al.  A revised methodology for research on metamemory: Pre-judgment Recall and Monitoring (PRAM). , 2004, Psychological methods.

[31]  F. Strack,et al.  Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. , 1991 .

[32]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Metacognition and Consciousness 1 Metacognition and Consciousness , 2006 .

[33]  Frank E. Ritter,et al.  What determines initial feeling of knowing? Familiarity with question terms , 1992 .

[34]  D. S. Lindsay,et al.  Remembering Mistaken for Knowing: Ease of Retrieval as a Basis for Confidence in Answers to General Knowledge Questions , 1993 .

[35]  Michaela Wänke,et al.  The effects of subjective ease of retrieval on attitudinal judgments : The moderating role of processing motivation , 2000 .

[36]  William F. Brewer,et al.  Confidence and accuracy in the recall of deceptive and nondeceptive sentences. , 2005 .

[37]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Importance of the kind of cue for judgments of learning (JOL) and the delayed-JOL effect , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[38]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Decisions about Ignorance: Knowing That You Don't Know. , 1981 .

[39]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Inflation of conditional predictions. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[40]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[41]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  An illusion of memory: false recognition influenced by unconscious perception , 1989 .

[42]  Michaela Wänke,et al.  There Are Many Reasons to Drive a BMW: Does Imagined Ease of Argument Generation Influence Attitudes? , 1997 .

[43]  L. Brooks,et al.  Nonanalytic Cognition: Memory, Perception, and Concept Learning , 1984 .

[44]  N. Schwarz,et al.  The Role of Ease of Retrieval and Attribution in Memory Judgments: Judging Your Memory as Worse Despite Recalling More Events , 1998 .

[45]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Reasons for confidence. , 1980 .

[46]  L. Reder,et al.  What determines initial feeling of knowing? Familiarity with question terms, not with the answer , 1992 .

[47]  N. Schwarz Metacognitive Experiences in Consumer Judgment and Decision Making , 2004 .

[48]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  The cue-familiarity heuristic in metacognition. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[49]  T. O. Nelson Metamemory: A Theoretical Framework and New Findings , 1990 .

[50]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  Cue familiarity but not target retrievability enhances feeling-of-knowing judgments. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[51]  S. Sloman The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .

[52]  L. Reder,et al.  Metacognition does not imply awareness: Strategy choice is governed by implicit learning and memory , 1996 .

[53]  F. Strack,et al.  Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[54]  F. Strack The different routes to social judgments: Experiential versus informational strategies , 1992 .

[55]  Asher Koriat,et al.  The Feeling of Knowing: Some Metatheoretical Implications for Consciousness and Control , 2000, Consciousness and Cognition.

[56]  D. Kahneman,et al.  A model of heuristic judgment , 2005 .

[57]  Guy Lories,et al.  Confidence Level and Feeling of Knowing in Question Answering - the Weight of Inferential Processes , 1992 .

[58]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Source monitoring. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.