The social grooming of captive female rhesus monkeys: Effects of the births of their infants

We observed the grooming interactions of 13 female rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)before and for 12 weeks after the births of their infants. Mothers groomed for similar amounts of time before and after the birth of their infants, but after the birth, the grooming they directed to their infants may have been at the expense of that directed to other partners. Lactating females did not receive more grooming from other females but were approached more often, suggesting that they were more attractive. Mothers that groomed their infants most groomed others least, as if grooming time was limited for each mother or as if she was trying to compensate for avoiding interactions with other partners. Mothers of male infants groomed others more than mothers with female infants did, which might be due to mothers with daughters receiving more aggression and therefore avoiding interaction. Experienced and high-ranking mothers groomed their newborn infants considerably more than primiparous mothers did in the 24 hr following birth. Grooming was preferentially directed at close kin before the births of the infants. Mothers tended to groom higher-ranked partners more than they were groomed by them, and they tended to receive more grooming from lower-ranked partners than they gave, as suggested in models of rank attractiveness.

[1]  J L FALK,et al.  The grooming behavior of the chimpanzee as a reinforcer. , 1958, Journal of The Experimental Analysis of Behavior.

[2]  E. Keverne Central mechanisms underlying the neural and neuroendocrine determinants of maternal behaviour , 1988, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[3]  J. Silk Altruism Among Female Macaca Radiata: Explanations and Analysis of Patterns of Grooming and Coalition Formation , 1982 .

[4]  Eduard Stammbach Group Responses To Specially Skilled Individuals in a Macaca Fascicularis Group , 1988 .

[5]  D. Maestripieri Vigilance Costs of Allogrooming in Macaque Mothers , 1993, The American Naturalist.

[6]  Robert M. Seyfarth,et al.  The distribution of grooming and related behaviours among adult female vervet monkeys , 1980, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  M. Simpson,et al.  Changes in the rhesus mother-infant relationship through the first four months of life , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  Eric B. Keverne,et al.  Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships , 1989, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[9]  Short-term consequences of different breeding histories for captive rhesus macaque mothers and young , 1985, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[10]  R. Hinde,et al.  Towards understanding relationships: Dynamic stability. , 1976 .

[11]  Martin Reite,et al.  On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque , 1989, Physiology & Behavior.

[12]  M. Boccia Comparison of the physical characteristics of grooming in two species of macaques (Macaca nemestrina and M. radiata). , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.

[13]  G. Hohmann,et al.  Male Loud Calls in Macaca silenus and Presbytis johnii , 1984 .

[14]  Joan B. Silk,et al.  The Influence of Kinship, Rank, and Sex On Affiliation and Aggression Between Adult Female and Immature Bonnet Macaques (Macaca Radiata) , 1981 .

[15]  C. M. Berman Mother-infant relationships among free-ranging rhesus monkeys on Cayo Santiago: A comparison with captive pairs , 1980, Animal Behaviour.

[16]  M. Simpson,et al.  Birth sex ratios and social rank in rhesus monkey mothers , 1982, Nature.

[17]  M. Simpson,et al.  Prior experience of risk and individual differences in enterprise shown by rhesus monkey infants in the second half of their first year , 1989, Primates.

[18]  R. Hinde,et al.  Changes in the Relationships of Captive Rhesus Monkeys On Giving Birth , 1977 .

[19]  J. Altmann,et al.  Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. , 1974, Behaviour.

[20]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Functional significance of social grooming in primates , 1991 .

[21]  C. Southwick,et al.  Structural Diversity and Mother-Infant Relations among Rhesus Monkeys in India and Nepal , 1984 .

[22]  D. Sade SOME ASPECTS OF PARENT-OFFSPRING AND SIBLING RELATIONS IN A GROUP OF RHESUS MONKEYS, WITH A DISCUSSION OF GROOMING. , 1965, American journal of physical anthropology.

[23]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  Toward a comparative socioecology of the genus Macaca: Different dominance styles in rhesus and stumptail monkeys , 1989, American journal of primatology.

[24]  J. Loy Behavioral responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage. , 1970, American journal of physical anthropology.

[25]  Robert Barton,et al.  Grooming site preferences in primates and their functional implications , 1985, International Journal of Primatology.

[26]  H. Kummer,et al.  On the value of social relationships to nonhuman primates: A heuristic scheme , 1978 .

[27]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  The similarity principle underlying social bonding among female rhesus monkeys. , 1986, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[28]  R. Hinde,et al.  “Aunt”-infant interaction in captive rhesus monkeys , 1964 .

[29]  W. Klemm,et al.  Oscillatory electrographic activity in the hippocampus: A mathematical model , 1980, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[30]  I. Bernstein,et al.  Social roles in a Rhesus monkey group. , 1966, Behaviour.

[31]  J. Panksepp,et al.  Endogenous opioids and social behavior , 1980, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[32]  T. Rowell,et al.  Reciprocity and partner preference in grooming of female blue monkeys , 1991, International Journal of Primatology.

[33]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Primate social systems , 1987 .

[34]  Robert M. Seyfarth,et al.  Social relationships among adult female baboons , 1976, Animal Behaviour.