Comparing simulator sickness in younger and older adults during simulated driving under different multisensory conditions

Abstract Driving simulators are valuable tools for traffic safety research as they allow for systematic reproductions of challenging situations that cannot be easily tested during real-world driving. Unfortunately, simulator sickness (i.e., nausea, dizziness, etc.) is common in many driving simulators and may limit their utility. The experience of simulator sickness is thought to be related to the sensory feedback provided to the user and is also thought to be greater in older compared to younger users. Therefore, the present study investigated whether adding auditory and/or motion cues to visual inputs in a driving simulator affected simulator sickness in younger and older adults. Fifty-eight healthy younger adults (age 18–39) and 63 healthy older adults (age 65+) performed a series of simulated drives under one of four sensory conditions: (1) visual cues alone, (2) combined visual + auditory cues (engine, tire, wind sounds), (3) combined visual + motion cues (via hydraulic hexapod motion platform), or (4) a combination of all three sensory cues (visual, auditory, motion). Simulator sickness was continuously recorded while driving and up to 15 min after driving session termination. Results indicated that older adults experienced more simulator sickness than younger adults overall and that females were more likely to drop out and drove for less time compared to males. No differences between sensory conditions were observed. However, older adults needed significantly longer time to fully recover from the driving session than younger adults, particularly in the visual-only condition. Participants reported that driving in the simulator was least realistic in the visual-only condition compared to the other conditions. Our results indicate that adding auditory and/or motion cues to the visual stimulus does not guarantee a reduction of simulator sickness per se, but might accelerate the recovery process, particularly in older adults.

[1]  J T Reason,et al.  Motion Sickness Adaptation: A Neural Mismatch Model 1 , 1978, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[2]  Lee Nolan,et al.  Aging, muscle activity, and balance control: physiologic changes associated with balance impairment. , 2003, Gait & posture.

[3]  C. Oman,et al.  Motion sickness: a synthesis and evaluation of the sensory conflict theory. , 1990, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[4]  John R. Wilson,et al.  Measurement of presence and its consequences in virtual environments , 2000, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[5]  Lawrence W. Stark,et al.  The Effects of Pictorial Realism, Delay of Visual Feedback, and Observer Interactivity on the Subjective Sense of Presence , 1996, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[6]  Robert S. Kennedy,et al.  Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. , 1993 .

[7]  Margaret D. Nolan,et al.  Vection and simulator sickness. , 1990, Military psychology : the official journal of the Division of Military Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[8]  Akiko Noda,et al.  Slower adaptation to driving simulator and simulator sickness in older adults , 2013 .

[9]  Joshua E. Domeyer,et al.  The use of adaptation to reduce simulator sickness in driving assessment and research. , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[10]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Perception of self-rotation (circular vection) induced by optokinetic stimuli. , 1972, Pflugers Archiv : European journal of physiology.

[11]  David A. Winter,et al.  Human balance and posture control during standing and walking , 1995 .

[12]  M. Wallace,et al.  Enhanced multisensory integration in older adults , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[13]  J. Golding Motion sickness susceptibility , 2006, Autonomic Neuroscience.

[14]  Mel Slater,et al.  Depth of Presence in Virtual Environments , 1994, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[15]  Hans-Peter Schöner,et al.  The Behavioral Validity of Dual-Task Driving Performance in Fixed and Moving Base Driving Simulators , 2016 .

[16]  Mark S Horswill,et al.  Auditory Feedback Influences Perceived Driving Speeds , 2008, Perception.

[17]  Gary E. Riccio,et al.  An Ecological Critique of the Sensory Conflict Theory of Motion Sickness , 1991 .

[18]  Jeannette R. Mahoney,et al.  Visual-somatosensory integration and balance: evidence for psychophysical integrative differences in aging. , 2014, Multisensory research.

[19]  J. Cummings,et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2005, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[20]  Benoît G. Bardy,et al.  Visually Induced Motion Sickness Predicted by Postural Instability , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[21]  Jennifer L. Mozolic,et al.  Multisensory Integration and Aging , 2012 .

[22]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Vection and visually induced motion sickness: how are they related? , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[23]  Nadia W. Mullen,et al.  Driving performance and susceptibility to simulator sickness: are they related? , 2010, The American journal of occupational therapy : official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association.

[24]  Michael E. McCauley,et al.  Cybersickness: Perception of Self-Motion in Virtual Environments , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[25]  Lori Ann Vallis,et al.  The relationship between postural stability and virtual environment adaptation , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[26]  H. Hecht,et al.  Visually induced motion sickness and presence in videogames: The role of sound , 2012 .

[27]  Carrie Heeter,et al.  Being There: The Subjective Experience of Presence , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[28]  Johnell O Brooks,et al.  Simulator sickness during driving simulation studies. , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[29]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Examining the Effect of Age on Visual–Vestibular Self-Motion Perception Using a Driving Paradigm , 2017, Perception.

[30]  Sherrilene Classen,et al.  Driving simulator sickness: an evidence-based review of the literature. , 2011, The American journal of occupational therapy : official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association.

[31]  Eric R. Muth,et al.  Characteristics of Head-Mounted Displays and Their Effects on Simulator Sickness , 2011 .

[32]  Matthew A. De Niear,et al.  Audiovisual Simultaneity Judgment and Rapid Recalibration throughout the Lifespan , 2016, PloS one.

[33]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Combined effects of auditory and visual cues on the perception of vection , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Julie M. Drexler,et al.  Motion sickness and proprioceptive aftereffects following virtual environment exposure. , 1999, Applied ergonomics.

[35]  Behrang Keshavarz,et al.  Illusory Self-motion in Virtual Environments , 2014 .

[36]  J. Golding Predicting individual differences in motion sickness susceptibility by questionnaire , 2006 .

[37]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Passive Restraint Reduces Visually Induced Motion Sickness in Older Adults , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[38]  R. Stern,et al.  Adaptation to vection-induced symptoms of motion sickness. , 1989, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[39]  Moira B. Flanagan,et al.  Sex differences in tolerance to visually-induced motion sickness. , 2005, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[40]  P S Cowings,et al.  Autogenic‐Feedback Training Exercise Is Superior to Promethazine for Control of Motion Sickness Symptoms , 2000, Journal of clinical pharmacology.

[41]  L. Burrows,et al.  Evaluating sensory conflict and postural instability. theories of motion sickness , 1998, Brain Research Bulletin.

[42]  Behrang Keshavarz,et al.  Validating an Efficient Method to Quantify Motion Sickness , 2011, Hum. Factors.

[43]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Age Differences in Visual-Auditory Self-Motion Perception during a Simulated Driving Task , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[44]  Bernhard E. Riecke,et al.  Moving sounds enhance the visually-induced self-motion illusion (circular vection) in virtual reality , 2009, TAP.

[45]  T. Stoffregen,et al.  An ecological Theory of Motion Sickness and Postural Instability , 1991 .

[46]  Eric Muth,et al.  The Challenge of Uncoupled Motion: Duration of Cognitive and Physiological Aftereffects , 2009, Hum. Factors.

[47]  Leonard Evans,et al.  Traffic Safety and the Driver , 1991 .

[48]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Demonstrating the Potential for Dynamic Auditory Stimulation to Contribute to Motion Sickness , 2014, PloS one.

[49]  Julie M. Drexler,et al.  Research in visually induced motion sickness. , 2010, Applied ergonomics.

[50]  Benoît G. Bardy,et al.  Stance Width Influences Postural Stability and Motion Sickness , 2010 .

[51]  Bob Cheung,et al.  Desensitization to strong vestibular stimuli improves tolerance to simulated aircraft motion. , 2005, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[52]  Geoffrey Hall,et al.  Effects of ethnicity and gender on motion sickness susceptibility. , 2005, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[53]  T. Stoffregen,et al.  Postural instability precedes motion sickness , 1998, Brain Research Bulletin.

[54]  Michel Guerraz,et al.  Mechanisms underlying visually induced body sway , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[55]  Gavriel Salvendy,et al.  Aftereffects and Sense of Presence in Virtual Environments: Formulation of a Research and Development Agenda , 1998, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[56]  Kay M. Stanney,et al.  What to Expect from Immersive Virtual Environment Exposure: Influences of Gender, Body Mass Index, and Past Experience , 2003, Hum. Factors.

[57]  S Q Hu,et al.  Effects of pre-exposures to a rotating optokinetic drum on adaptation to motion sickness. , 1991, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[58]  Maury A Nussbaum,et al.  Reliability of COP-based postural sway measures and age-related differences. , 2008, Gait & posture.

[59]  Bob G. Witmer,et al.  Human Performance in Virtual Environments: Effects of Presence, Immersive Tendency, and Simulator Sickness , 2004 .

[60]  Behrang Keshavarz,et al.  Intra-visual conflict in visually induced motion sickness , 2011, Displays.

[61]  Behrang Keshavarz,et al.  Stereoscopic Viewing Enhances Visually Induced Motion Sickness but Sound Does Not , 2012, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[62]  A. Väljamäe Auditorily-induced illusory self-motion: A review , 2009, Brain Research Reviews.