Examining the Consequences in the Tendency to Suppress and Reappraise Emotions on Task-Related Job Performance

This research tested the effects of individual differences in emotion regulation tendencies on task-related job performance and the mediating role of task focus. Emotion regulation has been divided into two broad classes, suppression and reappraisal, which may differentially relate to performance. By following self-regulation theories, it is believed that suppression requires more resources and will negatively relate to task performance via less task focus. Reappraisal requires fewer resources and should positively relate to performance via greater task focus. Results generally supported our expected relationships across both lab and field studies, and we discuss the theoretical and practical implications of our findings.

[1]  Gilad Chen,et al.  A multilevel integration of personality, climate, self-regulation, and performance. , 2006 .

[2]  C. Carver,et al.  Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control-Theory Approach to Human Behavior , 1981 .

[3]  J. Gross The Emerging Field of Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review , 1998 .

[4]  J. Gross,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Emotion Regulation and Memory: The Cognitive Costs of Keeping One's Cool , 2004 .

[5]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self-control resemble a muscle? , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  A. Tenbrunsel,et al.  Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes , 2013 .

[7]  Gilad Chen,et al.  A multilevel examination of the relationships among training outcomes, mediating regulatory processes, and adaptive performance. , 2005, The Journal of applied psychology.

[8]  D. Holman,et al.  Emotion regulation in customer service roles: testing a model of emotional labor. , 2003, Journal of occupational health psychology.

[9]  Raymond T. Lee,et al.  Testing a conservation of resources model of the dynamics of emotional labor. , 2002, Journal of occupational health psychology.

[10]  R. Kanfer Motivation theory and industrial and organizational psychology. , 1990 .

[11]  P. Shrout,et al.  Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. , 2002, Psychological methods.

[12]  D. Mackinnon Contrasts in multiple mediator models. , 2000 .

[13]  S. Gully,et al.  Role of goal orientation, ability, need for achievement, and locus of control in the self-efficacy and goal--setting process. , 1997 .

[14]  J. Gross,et al.  Emotion Regulation in Romantic Relationships: The Cognitive Consequences of Concealing Feelings , 2003 .

[15]  P. Ackerman,et al.  Goal setting, conditions of practice, and task performance: A resource allocation perspective. , 1994 .

[16]  Winston Bennett,et al.  Team Task Analysis: Identifying Tasks and Jobs That Are Team Based , 2005, Hum. Factors.

[17]  H. Weiss,et al.  An Examination of the Joint Effects of Affective Experiences and Job Beliefs on Job Satisfaction and Variations in Affective Experiences over Time. , 1999, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[18]  James J. Gross,et al.  Individual differences in emotion regulation , 2007 .

[19]  S. West,et al.  A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. , 2002, Psychological methods.

[20]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  James M. Diefendorff,et al.  Understanding the emotional labor process: A control theory perspective. , 2003 .

[22]  Bryan D. Edwards,et al.  An examination of factors contributing to a reduction in subgroup differences on a constructed-response paper-and-pencil test of scholastic achievement. , 2007, The Journal of applied psychology.

[23]  B. Schneider,et al.  A Framework for Analyzing Customer Service Orientations in Manufacturing , 1989 .

[24]  Gilad Chen,et al.  Examination of relationships among trait-like individual differences, state-like individual differences, and learning performance. , 2000, The Journal of applied psychology.

[25]  K. Vohs,et al.  Intellectual performance and ego depletion: role of the self in logical reasoning and other information processing. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  P. Ackerman,et al.  Motivation and cognitive abilities: an integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition , 1989 .

[27]  Eduardo Salas,et al.  Effects of Training Goals and Goal Orientation Traits on Multidimensional Training Outcomes and Performance Adaptability. , 2001, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[28]  D. Vandewalle Development and Validation of a Work Domain Goal Orientation Instrument , 1997 .

[29]  S. Hobfoll Conservation of resources. A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[30]  O. John,et al.  Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. , 2004, Journal of personality.

[31]  D. Beal,et al.  Episodic processes in emotional labor: perceptions of affective delivery and regulation strategies. , 2006, The Journal of applied psychology.

[32]  O. John,et al.  Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  Murray R. Barrick,et al.  Personality and job performance: test of the mediating effects of motivation among sales representatives. , 2002, The Journal of applied psychology.

[34]  D. Beal,et al.  An episodic process model of affective influences on performance. , 2005, The Journal of applied psychology.

[35]  Eleanor M. Smith,et al.  Relationships of goal orientation, metacognitive activity, and practice strategies with learning outcomes and transfer , 1998 .

[36]  A. Erez,et al.  The Role-Based Performance Scale: Validity Analysis of A Theory-Based Measure , 1998 .

[37]  Brian P. Flaherty,et al.  An Alternative Framework for Defining Mediation. , 1998, Multivariate behavioral research.

[38]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[39]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-control as limited resource: regulatory depletion patterns. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  James J. Gross,et al.  Composure at Any Cost? The Cognitive Consequences of Emotion Suppression , 1999 .

[41]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[42]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[43]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[44]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  J. Mathieu,et al.  Goal Orientation in Organizational Research: A Conceptual and Empirical Foundation , 1996 .

[46]  A. Baddeley Exploring the Central Executive , 1996 .

[47]  J. J. Donovan,et al.  The Role of State Goal Orientation in the Goal Establishment Process , 2005 .

[48]  Alicia A. Grandey,et al.  Emotion regulation in the workplace: a new way to conceptualize emotional labor. , 2000, Journal of occupational health psychology.

[49]  Gilad Chen,et al.  Development and validation of a work-specific measure of cognitive failure: Implications for occupational safety. , 2005 .

[50]  D. Mackinnon,et al.  Equivalence of the Mediation, Confounding and Suppression Effect , 2000, Prevention Science.