Fast mapping of multiple words: Insights into when ''the information provided'' does and does not equal ''the information perceived''

Abstract A candidate process for explaining the rapid vocabulary acquisition during the preschool years is “fast mapping,” children's ability to sketch partial maps of a word's meaning after brief exposure. The present study examines this process for learning multiple words, testing the hypothesis that children's attention to the information critical for quickly mapping multiple words onto their referents depends on the alternatives available when the words are introduced. Fifty-eight 40-month-old children participated in one of two conditions. In both conditions, each trial for novel Word #1 presented a novel object and three familiar ones. The conditions differed in the object choices presented when novel Word #2 was introduced. Although the same information was available to children in both conditions, younger children showed significantly better learning of the new words in the successive condition than in the concurrent condition. Implications of this for age-related differences and for teaching strategies are discussed.

[1]  E. Gibson,et al.  An Ecological Approach to Perceptual Learning and Development , 2000 .

[2]  S. Carey Semantic development : The state of the art , 1982 .

[3]  Nitin R. Patel,et al.  Exact logistic regression: theory and examples. , 1995, Statistics in medicine.

[4]  P. E. Morris,et al.  Practical aspects of memory , 1980 .

[5]  M. Rice Children's language acquisition. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[6]  W. Runquist,et al.  Learning and retention of paired adjectives as a function of percentage occurrence of response members. , 1960, Journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. , 1990 .

[8]  R. Magill,et al.  A REVIEW OF THE CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN MOTOR SKILL ACQUISITION , 1990 .

[9]  C. Dollaghan,et al.  Child meets word: "fast mapping" in preschool children. , 1985, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[10]  C. Mervis,et al.  Acquisition of the novel name-nameless category (N3C) principle by young children who have Down syndrome. , 1995, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[11]  E. Markman Categorization and naming in children , 1989 .

[12]  M L Rice,et al.  Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[13]  C. Mervis,et al.  Early object labels: the case for a developmental lexical principles framework , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[14]  Vito Modigliani,et al.  The effect of expanded versus massed practice on the retention of multiplication facts and spelling lists. , 1985 .

[15]  P. Spencer,et al.  Word-learning skills of deaf preschoolers: the development of novel mapping and rapid word-learning strategies. , 2000, Child development.

[16]  C R Mehta,et al.  The exact analysis of contingency tables in medical research , 1994, Statistical methods in medical research.

[17]  C. Mervis,et al.  Acquisition of the novel name--nameless category (N3C) principle. , 1994, Child development.

[18]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning , 1978 .

[19]  S. Soraci,et al.  Attentional functioning and relational learning. , 1990, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[20]  Jerome Kagan,et al.  The Second Year: The Emergence of Self-Awareness , 1981 .

[21]  J. Shea,et al.  Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. , 1979 .

[22]  David R. Olson,et al.  Cognitive development: The child's acquisition of diagonality , 1970 .

[23]  Eric Wanner,et al.  Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .

[24]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Processing in Human Memory , 1979 .

[25]  E. Bates,et al.  INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AND THEIR IMPLICATIONS FOR THEORIES OF LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT , 1995 .

[26]  Leslie M. Bailey,et al.  Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn New Nouns , 1992 .

[27]  Adaptations of fast mapping for vocabulary intervention with augmented language users , 2001 .

[28]  C. R. Mehta,et al.  StatXact : a statistical package for exact non-parametric inference, Cytel Software Corporation, Cambridge, MA, USA , 1990 .

[29]  R. Weisman,et al.  The effect of intermittent confirmation in programed instruction. , 1962 .

[30]  Susan Carey,et al.  Acquiring a Single New Word , 1978 .

[31]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  The learning of generalized motor programs: Reducing the relative frequency of knowledge of results enhances memory. , 1989 .

[32]  M. Rice,et al.  Fast mapping word-learning abilities of language-delayed preschoolers. , 1990, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[33]  D. C. Shapiro,et al.  Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition: support for the guidance hypothesis. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  David K. Dickinson,et al.  First impressions: Children’s knowledge of words gained from a single exposure , 1984, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[35]  Jane E. Clark,et al.  The Development of Movement Control and Coordination , 1982 .

[36]  Rebecca J. Brand,et al.  Breaking the language barrier: an emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. , 2000, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[37]  K. Wilkinson,et al.  Implications of fast mapping for vocabulary expansion in individuals with mental retardation , 1998 .

[38]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[39]  Catherine E. Snow,et al.  Children's language , 1990 .

[40]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  The Handbook of Child Language , 1995 .

[41]  E. Markman Constraints on word learning: Speculations about their nature, origins, and domain specificity. , 1992 .