Affective priming of semantic categorisation responses

Fazio, Sanbonmatsu Powell, & Kardes, (1986) demonstrated that less time is needed to affectively categorise a target as positive or negative when it is preceded by a prime with the same valence (e.g., summer-honest) compared to when the target is preceded by a prime with a different valence (e.g., cancer-honest). Such effects could be due to spreading of activation within a semantic network and/or to Stroop-like response conflicts. If a spreading of activation mechanism operates in priming tasks, primes should also facilitate nonaffective semantic processing of affectively congruent targets. In Experiment 1, we failed to observe affective priming when participants responded on the basis of whether the target referred to a person or animal. Experiment 2 revealed significant affective priming when participants responded on the basis of the valence of the targets but not when the semantic category of the targets (person or object) was relevant, despite the fact that apart from the task, both conditions were identical. The present results suggest that affective priming in the affective categorisation task is primarily due to the operation of a Stroop-like response conflict mechanism.

[1]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[2]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  Differences in the affective processing of words and pictures , 1994 .

[3]  K C Klauer,et al.  Does Sunshine Prime Loyal? Affective Priming in the Naming Task , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[4]  R. Fazio On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview , 2001 .

[5]  W. Glaser,et al.  Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  David M. Sanbonmatsu,et al.  On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  E. C. Dalrymple-Alford,et al.  Examination of Some Aspects of the Stroop Color-Word Test , 1966, Perceptual and motor skills.

[8]  K. C. Klauer,et al.  The Psychology of Evaluation : Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion , 2003 .

[9]  The Reverse Affect Test: A New Interference Task , 1985, Perceptual and motor skills.

[10]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Parts Outweigh the Whole (Word) in Unconscious Analysis of Meaning , 2000, Psychological science.

[11]  Roger Giner-Sorolla,et al.  The automatic evaluation of pictures , 1999 .

[12]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  A time course analysis of the affective priming effect , 2001 .

[13]  A. D. Groot,et al.  Primed Lexical Decision: Combined Effects of the Proportion of Related Prime-Target Pairs and the Stimulus-Onset Asynchrony of Prime and Target , 1984 .

[14]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[15]  J. Shelton,et al.  How semantic is automatic semantic priming? , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  D. Hermans,et al.  Affective priming using a color-naming task: a test of an affective-motivational account of affective priming effects. , 1998, Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie.

[17]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[18]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  M. Banaji,et al.  When fair is foul and foul is fair: reverse priming in automatic evaluation. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  Frank Baeyens,et al.  On the generality of the affective Simon effect , 2001 .

[21]  D. Balota,et al.  The locus of word-frequency effects in the pronunciation task: Lexical access and/or production? ☆ , 1985 .

[22]  D. Lowe,et al.  Selective and divided Attention in a Stroop task. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.

[23]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  An Affective Variant of the Simon Paradigm , 1998 .

[24]  J. Bargh The ecology of automaticity: toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[25]  W. Glaser Picture naming , 1992, Cognition.

[26]  Robert E. Haussmann Tachistoscopic presentation and millisecond timing on the IBM PC/XT/AT and PS/2: A Turbo Pascal unit to provide general-purpose routines for CGA, Hercules, EGA, and VGA monitors , 1992 .

[27]  E. Stern,et al.  How attitudes guide memory-based judgments: A two-process model , 1992 .

[28]  R. E. Warren,et al.  Association, directionality, and stimulus encoding. , 1974 .

[29]  K. C. Klauer,et al.  List-context effects in evaluative priming. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  M. Masson A distributed memory model of semantic priming. , 1995 .

[31]  Dirk Wentura,et al.  Activation and Inhibition of Affective Information: for Negative Priming in the Evaluation Task , 1999 .

[32]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  Affective priming of pronunciation responses: effects of target degradation , 2001 .

[33]  J. H. Neely,et al.  Semantic Context Effects on Visual Word Processing: A Hybrid Prospective-Retrospective Processing Theory , 1989 .

[34]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The Automatic Evaluation Effect: Unconditional Automatic Attitude Activation with a Pronunciation Task , 1996 .

[35]  Cynthia P. May,et al.  Determinants of negative priming. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[36]  D. Holender,et al.  Expectancy effects, congruity effects and the interpretation of response latency measurement , 1992 .

[37]  M. Braga,et al.  Exploratory Data Analysis , 2018, Encyclopedia of Social Network Analysis and Mining. 2nd Ed..

[38]  D. Wentura Affektives Priming in der Wortentscheidungsaufgabe: Evidenz für postlexikalische Urteilstendenzen , 1998 .

[39]  G. Logan,et al.  When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .

[40]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[41]  M. Klinger,et al.  Mechanisms of unconscious priming: I. Response competition, not spreading activation. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[42]  E. Fox Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[43]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A retrieval theory of priming in memory. , 1988, Psychological review.

[44]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Source of Inhibition in Experiments on the Effect of Sentence Context on Word Recognition. , 1982 .

[45]  Gordon H. Bower,et al.  Mood Congruity of Social Judgments , 1991 .

[46]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  IBM PC/XT/AT and PS/2 Turbo Pascal timing with extended resolution , 1990 .

[47]  D. Wentura,et al.  Dissociative affective and associative priming effects in the lexical decision task: yes versus no responses to word targets reveal evaluative judgment tendencies. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[48]  D. Hermans,et al.  Affective and subjective familiarity ratings of 740 Dutch words , 1994 .

[49]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  Affective and Identity Priming with Episodically Associated Stimuli , 1998 .

[50]  D. Hermans,et al.  The affective priming effect: Automatic activation of evaluative information in memory. , 1994 .

[51]  Julie E. Boland,et al.  Priming in pronunciation: Beyond pattern recognition and onset latency , 1989 .

[52]  R. Schreuder,et al.  Semantic priming and Stroop-like interference in word-naming tasks. , 1985 .