Evolutionary perspectives on technology and technological change

It is my view that many so-called theoretical disputes in archaeology stem from the fact that participants in the debates are actually trying to answer different questions. Thus, I begin this essay with a brief discussion of the problems that most occupy my attention. My main research area is the Pleistocene prehistory of western Eurasia, although huntergatherer studies, both archaeological and ethnographic, inform my research on the Middle and Upper Paleolithic. I am especially interested in how the role of technology in human life has changed over the past 2.5 million years. Over that time artifacts have evolved from simple extensions of the human physical apparatus and relatively peripheral elements in the behavioral repertoire of the genus Homo to a central component in practically every interaction between humans and their environments as well as between humans and other humans. In short, artifacts have been transformed from simple extensions of hands, claws and teeth into material culture. This is obviously a long and complex story, but it is also central to understanding the human condition and how it came about. Evolutionary concepts and models provide some of the best tools for learning about the kinds of long-term processes that engage my interest. Many archaeologists also find them useful for examining processes that unfolded over much shorter time frames, even within the span of a single human generation. In this essay I explore the rationale for applying evolutionary ideas to the study of technology, and some of the ways these ideas have been used. I would not claim to speak for everyone who flies the banner of evolution in archaeology, for they are a diverse and contentious lot. Instead I focus on what seem to be the most productive rationales and approaches. Speaking at the broadest level, concepts and models derived from evolutionary theory have proven or could prove useful in three general contexts. One obvious application arises where there is thought to be a direct connection between technological and biological changes occurring on an evolutionary time scale. The most salient examples are arguments linking changes in artifact form or manufacture techniques to evolutionary developments in human cognition (Coolidge and Wynn 2001; Mithen 1996, 1997; Wynn

[1]  P. Jeffrey Brantingham,et al.  A Neutral Model of Stone Raw Material Procurement , 2003, American Antiquity.

[2]  T. Jukes,et al.  The neutral theory of molecular evolution. , 2000, Genetics.

[3]  Todd A. Surovell,et al.  The Tortoise and the Hare , 2000, Current Anthropology.

[4]  Marcia-Anne Dobres,et al.  Agency in Archaeology , 2000 .

[5]  R. Elston,et al.  Thinking outside the box: A new perspective on diet breadth and sexual division of labor in the Prearchaic Great Basin , 2002, World archaeology.

[6]  Robert L. Kelly,et al.  The Foraging Spectrum , 2007 .

[7]  Michelle Hegmon,et al.  Setting Theoretical Egos Aside: Issues and Theory in North American Archaeology , 2003, American Antiquity.

[8]  T. Wynn Piaget, stone tools and the evolution of human intelligence. , 1985, World archaeology.

[9]  R. Foley Hominid species and stone-tool assemblages: how are they related? , 1987, Antiquity.

[10]  M. Flinn Culture and the evolution of social learning , 1997 .

[11]  Andrew M. Colman,et al.  The complexity of cooperation: Agent-based models of competition and collaboration , 1998, Complex..

[12]  Marcia-Anne Dobres,et al.  Technology and Social Agency: Outlining a Practice Framework for Archaeology , 1991 .

[13]  C. Bousman,et al.  Hunter-Gatherer Adaptations, Economic Risk and Tool Design , 1993 .

[14]  S. Mithen,et al.  Handaxes: products of sexual selection? , 1999, Antiquity.

[15]  E. Smith,et al.  Is It Evolution Yet? A Critique of Evolutionary Archaeology1 , 1998, Current Anthropology.

[16]  Peter Bleed,et al.  The Optimal Design of Hunting Weapons: Maintainability or Reliability , 1986, American Antiquity.

[17]  Pierre Lemonnier,et al.  Elements for an Anthropology of Technology , 1992 .

[18]  R. H. Halperin,et al.  Ecology and Mode of Production: Seasonal Variation and the Division of Labor by Sex among Hunter-Gatherers , 1980, Journal of Anthropological Research.

[19]  Heidi Knecht,et al.  Projectile Points of Bone, Antler, and Stone Experimental Explorations of Manufacture and Use , 1997 .

[20]  Stephen Shennan,et al.  Cultural Transmission and Stochastic Network Growth , 2003, American Antiquity.

[21]  N. Gotelli,et al.  NULL MODELS IN ECOLOGY , 1996 .

[22]  Robert A. Foley,et al.  On stony ground: Lithic technology, human evolution, and the emergence of culture , 2003 .

[23]  M. Dobres,et al.  Gender and prehistoric technology: On the social agency of technical strategies , 1995 .

[24]  T. Wynn Two Developments in the Mind of Early Homo , 1993 .

[25]  Steven L. Kuhn,et al.  Middle palaeolithic ‘creativity’: Reflections on an oxymoron? , 2005 .

[26]  Wendell H. Oswalt,et al.  An anthropological analysis of food-getting technology , 1976 .

[27]  Timothy A. Kohler,et al.  Dynamics in human and primate societies: agent-based modeling of social and spatial processes , 2000 .

[28]  Kim Hill,et al.  Female subsistence strategies among Ache hunter-gatherers of Eastern Paraguay , 1985 .

[29]  Amots Zehavi,et al.  The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin's Puzzle , 1997 .

[30]  Julie C. Stout,et al.  Stone Tool-Making and Brain Activation: Position Emission Tomography (PET) Studies , 2000 .

[31]  Robert L. Bettinger,et al.  Point Typologies, Cultural Transmission, and the Spread of Bow-and-Arrow Technology in the Prehistoric Great Basin , 1999, American Antiquity.

[32]  Todd A. Surovell,et al.  Paleolithic population growth pulses evidenced by small animal exploitation , 1999, Science.

[33]  T. Wynn,et al.  Executive Functions of the Frontal Lobes and the Evolutionary Ascendancy of Homo Sapiens , 2001 .

[34]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Showing off: Tests of an hypothesis about men's foraging goals , 1991 .

[35]  E. Charnov Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. , 1976, Theoretical population biology.

[36]  R. L. Linscheid,et al.  EMG study of hand muscle recruitment during hard hammer percussion manufacture of Oldowan tools. , 1998, American journal of physical anthropology.

[37]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Hadza Women's Time Allocation, Offspring Provisioning, and the Evolution of Long Postmenopausal Life Spans , 1997, Current Anthropology.

[38]  A. Ugan,et al.  The effect of handling time on subsistence technology , 2002 .

[39]  R. L. Kelly The Foraging Spectrum: Diversity in Hunter-Gatherer Lifeways , 1997 .

[40]  Jason Bright,et al.  When is technology worth the trouble , 2003 .

[41]  R. Bird,et al.  The Ethnoarchaeology of Juvenile Foragers: Shellfishing Strategies among Meriam Children , 2000 .

[42]  K. Sassaman Lithic Technology and the Hunter-Gatherer Sexual Division of Labor , 1993 .

[43]  S. Mithen The prehistory of the mind: a search for the origins of art, religion and science , 1999 .