Producing Pronouns and Definite Noun Phrases: Do Speakers Use the Addressee's Discourse Model?
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Martin J Pickering,et al. The use of visual context during the production of referring expressions , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[2] Barbara J. Grosz,et al. Pronouns, Names, and the Centering of Attention in Discourse , 1993, Cogn. Sci..
[3] Matthew P. Aylett,et al. Referential form, duration, and modelling the listener in dialogue. , 2001 .
[4] Matthew Stone,et al. Sentence generation as a planning problem , 2007, ACL.
[5] T. Givón. Topic continuity in discourse: An introduction , 1983 .
[6] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. The effect of additional characters on choice of referring expression: Everyone counts. , 2007, Journal of memory and language.
[7] M. Aylett,et al. Language redundancy predicts syllabic duration and the spectral characteristics of vocalic syllable nuclei. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[8] A. Almor,et al. Noun-phrase anaphors and focus: the informational load hypothesis. , 1999, Psychological review.
[9] Thomas Hofmann,et al. Speakers optimize information density through syntactic reduction , 2007 .
[10] Wilbert Spooren,et al. Text representation : linguistic and psycholinguistic aspects , 2001 .
[11] Mija M. Van Der Wege. Lexical Entrainment and Lexical Differentiation in Reference Phrase Choice. , 2009 .
[12] Roger P. G. van Gompel,et al. Choosing anaphoric expressions : do people take into account likelihood of reference? , 2010 .
[13] Hua Cheng,et al. Integrating Referring and Informing in NP Planning , 1998 .
[14] A J Schafer,et al. Intonational Disambiguation in Sentence Production and Comprehension , 2000, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[15] Jeanette K. Gundel,et al. Cognitive Status and the form of Referring Expressions in Discourse , 1993, The Oxford Handbook of Reference.
[16] William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al. Discourse: The Establishment and Maintenance of Reference , 2022 .
[17] Graeme Hirst,et al. Collaborating on Referring Expressions , 1991, CL.
[18] 付伶俐. 打磨Using Language,倡导新理念 , 2014 .
[19] H. H. Clark,et al. Audience Design in Meaning and Reference , 1982 .
[20] Rainer Bromme,et al. Lexical Entrainment in Written Discourse: Is Experts' Word Use Adapted to the Addressee? , 2008 .
[21] Siobhan Chapman. Logic and Conversation , 2005 .
[22] Robert Dale,et al. Computational Interpretations of the Gricean Maxims in the Generation of Referring Expressions , 1995, Cogn. Sci..
[23] S. Brennan,et al. Addressees' needs influence speakers' early syntactic choices , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[24] R. Gerrig,et al. Speakers’ experiences and audience design: knowing when and knowing how to adjust utterances to addressees☆ , 2002 .
[25] D. Barr,et al. Perspective taking and the coordination of meaning in language use , 2006 .
[26] C. Roßnagel,et al. Lost in thought: cognitive load and the processing of addressees' feedback in verbal communication. , 2004, Experimental Psychology.
[27] Douglas E. Appelt,et al. Planning English Referring Expressions , 1985, Artif. Intell..
[28] Douglas E. Appelt,et al. Some Pragmatic Issues in the Planning of Definite & Indefinite Noun Phrases , 1985, ACL.
[29] W. Chafe. Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view , 1976 .
[30] S. Brennan,et al. Attenuating Information in Spoken Communication: For the Speaker, or for the Addressee?. , 2010 .
[31] Kumiko Fukumura,et al. The effect of animacy on the choice of referring expression , 2011 .
[32] W. Levelt,et al. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .
[33] Dan Jurafsky,et al. Effects of disfluencies, predictability, and utterance position on word form variation in English conversation. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[34] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. The Effect of Thematic Roles on Pronoun Use and Frequency of Reference Continuation , 2001 .
[35] Douglas E. Appelt,et al. A Computational Model of Referring , 1987, IJCAI.
[36] D. Barr,et al. Limits on theory of mind use in adults , 2003, Cognition.
[37] Kathryn Bock,et al. Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .
[38] Roger P. G. van Gompel,et al. How does similarity-based interference affect the choice of referring expression? , 2011 .
[39] T. Florian Jaeger,et al. Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.
[40] H. H. Clark,et al. Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[41] William D. Raymond,et al. Probabilistic Relations between Words: Evidence from Reduction in Lexical Production , 2008 .
[42] R. Gerrig,et al. The impact of memory demands on audience design during language production , 2005, Cognition.
[43] William S. Horton,et al. Conversational Common Ground and Memory Processes in Language Production , 2005 .
[44] Mira Ariel. Accessibility theory: An overview , 2001 .
[45] M. Tanenhaus,et al. Approaches to studying world-situated language use : bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions , 2005 .
[46] Rosemary J. Stevenson,et al. Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events , 1994 .
[47] Julie C. Sedivy,et al. Evidence of Perspective-Taking Constraints in Children's On-Line Reference Resolution , 2002, Psychological science.
[48] Christian Nagel. Cognitive load and perspective‐taking: applying the automatic‐controlled distinction to verbal communication , 2000 .
[49] S. Brennan,et al. Prosodic disambiguation of syntactic structure: For the speaker or for the addressee? , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.
[50] Jerome A. Niles,et al. The Context of Comprehension , 1981 .
[51] Anna L. Theakston,et al. The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children's use of referring expressions , 2006, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[52] T. Givón,et al. The grammar of referential coherence as mental processing instructions , 1992 .
[53] Alice Turk,et al. The Smooth Signal Redundancy Hypothesis: A Functional Explanation for Relationships between Redundancy, Prosodic Prominence, and Duration in Spontaneous Speech , 2004, Language and speech.
[54] Herbert H. Clark,et al. Coordinating beliefs in conversation , 1992 .
[55] G. Dell,et al. Effect of Ambiguity and Lexical Availability on Syntactic and Lexical Production , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[56] V. Ferreira,et al. Speaker-external versus speaker-internal forces on utterance form: do cognitive demands override threats to referential success? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[57] William D. Raymond,et al. The effects of collocational strength and contextual predictability in lexical production 1 , 1999 .
[58] Gary S. Dell,et al. Mechanisms for Listener-Adaptation in Language Production: Limiting the Role of the "Model of the Listener" , 2013 .
[59] C. Fletcher. Markedness and topic continuity in discourse processing , 1984 .
[60] Emiel Krahmer,et al. Toward a Computational Psycholinguistics of Reference Production , 2012, Top. Cogn. Sci..
[61] J. Trueswell,et al. Using prosody to avoid ambiguity: Effects of speaker awareness and referential context , 2003 .
[62] M. Pickering,et al. Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[63] A Pollatsek,et al. On the use of counterbalanced designs in cognitive research: a suggestion for a better and more powerful analysis. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[64] Victor S. Ferreira,et al. How do speakers avoid ambiguous linguistic expressions? , 2005, Cognition.
[65] Scott Weinstein,et al. Centering: A Framework for Modeling the Local Coherence of Discourse , 1995, CL.
[66] B. J. Winer. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .
[67] Jeanette K. Gundel,et al. Cognitive Status and the Form of Referring Expressions in Discourse , 1993 .
[68] Kenneth I Forster,et al. DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[69] R. Ratcliff,et al. Reliability of prosodic cues for resolving syntactic ambiguity. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[70] H. H. Clark,et al. References in Conversation Between Experts and Novices , 1987 .
[71] S. Brennan. Centering Attention in Discourse. , 1995 .
[72] Christian Nagel. Cognitive load and perspective‐taking: applying the automatic‐controlled distinction to verbal communication , 2000 .
[73] B. Keysar,et al. When do speakers take into account common ground? , 1996, Cognition.
[74] G. Dell,et al. Adapting production to comprehension: The explicit mention of instruments , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[75] Ellen Gurman Bard,et al. The effect of repetition on words in recorded dictations , 1989, EUROSPEECH.
[76] Mira Ariel. Accessing Noun-Phrase Antecedents , 1990 .
[77] E. Prince. Fancy syntax and ‘shared knowledge’ , 1985 .