Dissociating Number Line Estimations from Underlying Numerical Representations

Estimation patterns in the number line task are usually interpreted to indicate the (logarithmic or linear) nature of the underlying mental number line. However, indicators of the to-be-achieved linear representation may also be confounded with task requirements or strategies to achieve optimal task performance. In this study, we dissociated correct task performance from indices of a linear representation. Therefore, we designed an experiment in which adults and first graders had to learn number-to-space mappings of nonlinear functions. For adults, we found better fits of the corresponding functions than a linear function after just a few minutes of training. For most first graders, estimation patterns were not fitted better by a logarithmic function when they had to learn a logarithmic layout. Thus, estimation patterns produced in the number line estimation task do not necessarily allow for valid inferences on the underlying representation of number magnitude.

[1]  Charles Chree,et al.  The Relationship between the , 1925 .

[2]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale , 2001, Psychological science.

[3]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.

[4]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[5]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[6]  H. Barth,et al.  Developmental change in numerical estimation. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[7]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[8]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Representational change and children’s numerical estimation , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  P. Onghena,et al.  The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5- to 9-year old children: evidence for a segmented linear model. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  F. Restle Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .

[11]  R. Church,et al.  Time left: linear versus logarithmic subjective time. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[12]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. , 2007, Child development.

[13]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes along the right lines: differentiating between scale and bias. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[15]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  Children's early mental number line: logarithmic or decomposed linear? , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[16]  I. Spence Visual psychophysics of simple graphical elements. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  Language Effects on Children’s Nonverbal Number Line Estimations , 2011 .

[18]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Development of Numerical Estimation: A Review , 2004 .

[19]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Comparing apples and pears in studies on magnitude estimations , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[20]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  D. Cohen,et al.  Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  Hilary C Barth,et al.  The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.

[23]  R M Church,et al.  Time left: linear versus logarithmic subjective time. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[24]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Linear Numerical-Magnitude Representations Aid Children’s Memory for Numbers , 2010, Psychological science.

[25]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of consciousness , 2001 .

[26]  David R. Anderson,et al.  AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: some background, observations, and comparisons , 2011, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[27]  Barbara J. Juhasz,et al.  Adults’ number-line estimation strategies: Evidence from eye movements , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  S. Lazic,et al.  Model based Inference in the Life Sciences: a Primer on Evidence , 2011 .

[29]  R. Siegler,et al.  Development of Quantitative Thinking , 2012 .

[30]  Brian P. Dyre,et al.  Bias in proportion judgments: the cyclical power model. , 2000, Psychological review.

[31]  R. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.

[32]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. , 2008, Child development.