The role of frequency resolution and temporal resolution in the detection of frequency modulation.

The experiment investigated subjects' ability to detect short-duration changes in frequency. In an adaptive, 2AFC task, three normal-hearing subjects were asked to distinguish a sinusoidal signal that increased in frequency in a series of discrete steps from a standard that was identical except that its frequency increased essentially continuously. The signals were 60 ms in duration with center frequencies of 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 3, 4, and 6 kHz. The smallest frequency increase between steps (FI) at which the stepped signal could be distinguished from the standard was determined as a function of the number of steps in the signal. As the number of steps increased and the step duration decreased, the FI at first decreased and then reached a roughly asymptotic level. Eventually, however, at a certain number of steps, the FI increased rapidly. The data were analyzed using a model of auditory temporal resolution that included a bank of bandpass filters, a nonlinearity, a temporal integrator, and a decision device. The analysis yielded ERDs that ranged from 3.8 to 5.0 ms and did not change systematically with frequency. Detector efficiency varied considerably, being greatest at 0.5 and 1 kHz, and declining at higher and lower center frequencies.

[1]  David A. Huffman,et al.  The generation of impulse-equivalent pulse trains , 1962, IRE Trans. Inf. Theory.

[2]  R. Plomp Rate of Decay of Auditory Sensation , 1964 .

[3]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[4]  J. E. Rose,et al.  Phase-locked response to low-frequency tones in single auditory nerve fibers of the squirrel monkey. , 1967, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  D. M. Green,et al.  Discrimination of transient signals having identical energy spectra. , 1970, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  David J. Anderson,et al.  Temporal Position of Discharges in Single Auditory Nerve Fibers within the Cycle of a Sine‐Wave Stimulus: Frequency and Intensity Effects , 1971 .

[7]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  B. Moore Frequency difference limens for short-duration tones. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  W Jesteadt,et al.  Temporal acuity in listeners with sensorineural hearing loss. , 1976, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[10]  B. Moore An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing , 1977 .

[11]  N. Viemeister Temporal modulation transfer functions based upon modulation thresholds. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  R. Kay Hearing of modulation in sounds. , 1982, Physiological reviews.

[13]  B C Moore,et al.  Gap detection as a function of frequency, bandwidth, and level. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  P. Fitzgibbons,et al.  Temporal gap detection in noise as a function of frequency, bandwidth, and level. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  B. Moore,et al.  Detection of temporal gaps in bandlimited noise: effects of variations in bandwidth and signal-to-masker ratio. , 1985, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  N. Viemeister,et al.  Temporal modulation transfer functions in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. , 1985, Audiology : official organ of the International Society of Audiology.

[17]  N. Viemeister,et al.  Simultaneous masking by gated and continuous sinusoidal maskers. , 1985, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  Brian C. J. Moore,et al.  The relationship between frequency selectivity and frequency discrimination for subjects with unilateral and bilateral cochlear impairments , 1986 .

[19]  B. Moore,et al.  Gap detection and the auditory filter: phase effects using sinusoidal stimuli. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  D. M. Green,et al.  Detection of partially filled gaps in noise and the temporal modulation transfer function. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  B C Moore,et al.  The shape of the ear's temporal window. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  Auditory temporal acuity for dynamic signals , 1989 .

[23]  Brian C. J. Moore,et al.  Mechanisms underlying the frequency discrimination of pulsed tones and the detection of frequency modulation , 1989 .

[24]  B. Moore,et al.  Temporal window shape as a function of frequency and level. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  Brian R Glasberg,et al.  Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data , 1990, Hearing Research.

[26]  B. Moore,et al.  Decrement detection in normal and impaired ears. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  T. G. Forrest,et al.  Detection of silent temporal gaps in sinusoidal markers. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  L. Feth,et al.  Temporal resolution in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners using frequency-modulated stimuli. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[29]  R. Patterson,et al.  Complex Sounds and Auditory Images , 1992 .

[30]  J H Grose,et al.  The detection of temporal gaps as a function of frequency region and absolute noise bandwidth. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  B C Moore,et al.  Detection of temporal gaps in sinusoids: effects of frequency and level. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[32]  David A. Eddins,et al.  Amplitude modulation detection of narrow‐band noise: Effects of absolute bandwidth and frequency region , 1993 .