Rapid Interactions between Lexical Semantic and Word Form Analysis during Word Recognition in Context: Evidence from ERPs
暂无分享,去创建一个
Albert Kim | Vicky Lai | A. Kim | V. Lai | V. T. Lai
[1] S. Geisser,et al. On methods in the analysis of profile data , 1959 .
[2] M. Tovée,et al. Processing speed in the cerebral cortex and the neurophysiology of visual masking , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[3] G. V. Simpson,et al. Flow of activation from V1 to frontal cortex in humans , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] J. Pernier,et al. ERP Manifestations of Processing Printed Words at Different Psycholinguistic Levels: Time Course and Scalp Distribution , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[5] I. staff. Front end , 2002 .
[6] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .
[7] L. Aravind,et al. Integration of Word Meaning and World Knowledge in Language Comprehension , 2022 .
[8] C. C. Wood,et al. Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[9] S. Dehaene,et al. The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus , 2002, Neuroreport.
[10] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. Early semantic context integration and lexical access as revealed by event-related brain potentials , 2007, Biological Psychology.
[11] Michael Grosvald,et al. Lexical access in American Sign Language: An ERP investigation of effects of semantics and phonology , 2012, Brain Research.
[12] William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al. The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data , 2006, NeuroImage.
[13] Thomas A. Farmer,et al. Early Occipital Sensitivity to Syntactic Category Is Based on Form Typicality , 2010, Psychological science.
[14] M. Sigman,et al. Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2022 .
[15] Heinz Wimmer,et al. When does the brain register deviances from standard word spellings?--An ERP study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[16] R. Salmelin,et al. Distinct time courses of word and context comprehension in the left temporal cortex. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[17] M. Pickering,et al. An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[18] K. Rayner,et al. Measuring word recognition in reading: eye movements and event-related potentials , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[19] Nancy Ide,et al. The American National Corpus , 2004 .
[20] John J. Foxe,et al. Determinants and mechanisms of attentional modulation of neural processing. , 2001, Frontiers in Bioscience.
[21] Katherine A. DeLong,et al. Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[22] Suzanne Dikker,et al. Before the N400: Effects of lexical–semantic violations in visual cortex , 2011, Brain and Language.
[23] V. Lamme,et al. The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.
[24] Mariano Sigman,et al. Hierarchical Coding of Letter Strings in the Ventral Stream: Dissecting the Inner Organization of the Visual Word-Form System , 2007, Neuron.
[25] Ellen F. Lau,et al. Dissociating N400 Effects of Prediction from Association in Single-word Contexts , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[26] F. Pulvermüller,et al. Effects of word length and frequency on the human event-related potential , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[27] R. O’Reilly. Six principles for biologically based computational models of cortical cognition , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[28] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Watching the Word Go by: On the Time-course of Component Processes in Visual Word Recognition , 2009, Lang. Linguistics Compass.
[29] M. Kutas,et al. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.
[30] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. A Beautiful Day in the Neighborhood: An Event-Related Potential Study of Lexical Relationships and Prediction in Context. , 2009, Journal of memory and language.
[31] C. Schroeder,et al. A spatiotemporal profile of visual system activation revealed by current source density analysis in the awake macaque. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.
[32] Peter Indefrey,et al. Monitoring in language perception: Electrophysiological and hemodynamic responses to spelling violations , 2011, NeuroImage.
[33] Bruce D. McCandliss,et al. The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[34] H. Rabagliati,et al. Sensitivity to syntax in visual cortex , 2009, Cognition.
[35] J. Grainger,et al. ERP Evidence for Ultra-Fast Semantic Processing in the Picture–Word Interference Paradigm , 2010, Front. Psychology.
[36] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Masked repetition and translation priming in second language learners: a window on the time-course of form and meaning activation using erps. , 2009, Psychophysiology.
[37] James L. McClelland,et al. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.
[38] L. Pylkkänen,et al. Neuromagnetic Evidence for the Timing of Lexical Activation: An MEG Component Sensitive to Phonotactic Probability but Not to Neighborhood Density , 2002, Brain and Language.
[39] M. Posner,et al. Establishing a time‐line of word recognition: evidence from eye movements and event‐related potentials , 1998, Neuroreport.
[40] Cathy J. Price,et al. Top-down modulation of ventral occipito-temporal responses during visual word recognition , 2011, NeuroImage.
[41] L. Osterhout,et al. Neural correlates of second-language word learning: minimal instruction produces rapid change , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[42] H. Semlitsch,et al. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[43] Bruno Rossion,et al. The Speed of Orthographic Processing during Lexical Decision: Electrophysiological Evidence for Independent Coding of Letter Identity and Letter Position in Visual Word Recognition , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[44] J. Moake,et al. This article has been cited by other articles , 2003 .
[45] J. Grainger,et al. An Electrophysiological Study of the Effects of Orthographic Neighborhood Size on Printed Word Perception , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[46] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .
[47] T. Allison,et al. Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe , 1994, Nature.
[48] A. Jacobs,et al. Event-Related Potentials Reveal Rapid Verification of Predicted Visual Input , 2009, PloS one.
[49] R. Salmelin,et al. Dynamics of letter string perception in the human occipitotemporal cortex. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[50] P. Holcomb. Semantic priming and stimulus degradation: implications for the role of the N400 in language processing. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[51] Bruce D. McCandliss,et al. Fast, visual specialization for reading in English revealed by the topography of the N170 ERP response , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[52] Manuel Carreiras,et al. Long-range neural synchronization supports fast and efficient reading: EEG correlates of processing expected words in sentences , 2013, NeuroImage.
[53] Bruno Rossion,et al. Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.
[54] Jana Straková,et al. Concurrent effects of lexical status and letter-rotation during early stage visual word recognition: Evidence from ERPs , 2012, Brain Research.
[55] David Huepe,et al. What event-related potentials (ERPs) bring to social neuroscience? , 2012, Social neuroscience.
[56] Shimon Ullman,et al. Image interpretation by a single bottom-up top-down cycle , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[57] Hartmut Fitz,et al. Getting real about Semantic Illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension , 2012, Brain Research.
[58] G. Stone,et al. Perception Is a Two-Way Street: Feedforward and Feedback Phonology in Visual Word Recognition ☆ , 1997 .
[59] S. Sereno,et al. Context Effects in Word Recognition , 2003, Psychological science.
[60] H. Kolk,et al. Monitoring in language perception: The effect of misspellings of words in highly constrained sentences , 2006, Brain Research.
[61] T. Poggio,et al. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[62] Donna Coch,et al. Word and pseudoword superiority effects reflected in the ERP waveform , 2010, Brain Research.
[63] H. Ferguson,et al. Galvanic vestibular stimulation modulates the electrophysiological response during face processing , 2012, Visual Neuroscience.
[64] Jelena Mirkovic,et al. Incrementality and Prediction in Human Sentence Processing , 2009, Cogn. Sci..
[65] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Computing the meanings of words in reading: cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. , 2004, Psychological review.
[66] L. Pylkkänen,et al. Tracking the time course of word recognition with MEG , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[67] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. , 1982, Psychological review.
[68] Alec Marantz,et al. Lexical access in early stages of visual word processing: A single-trial correlational MEG study of heteronym recognition , 2009, Brain and Language.
[69] M. Tovée. Neuronal Processing: How fast is the speed of thought? , 1994, Current Biology.