No Evidence for Accurate Visuomotor Memory: Systematic and Variable Error in Memory-Guided Reaching

Abstract The authors explored whether the motor system has access to highly accurate information about the aiming environment after visual occlusion. Participants (N = 14) reached to 1 of 3 midsagittal targets in 4 visual conditions (open-loop, brief-delay, 500-ms delay, and 2,000-ms delay). In all conditions, the aiming environment was first viewed for 2,000 ms. Movements were cued immediately after the initial viewing period in the open-loop and brief-delay conditions. Vision was not occluded until movement onset in the open-loop condition, whereas vision was occluded coincidentally with the movement cue in the brief-delay condition. In the 2 longer delay conditions, the movement was cued following a 500- or a 2,000-ms no-vision delay period. Participants overshot the target in the open-loop condition, but that tendency was significantly reduced in the 3 delay conditions. Moreover, end-point variability was greater in the 3 delay conditions than in the open-loop condition. A speed-accuracy tradeoff account could not explain the differences between open-loop and delayed reaching. Those findings suggest that the motor system does not have access to highly accurate information about the aiming environment for any appreciable period of time following visual occlusion, consistent with the view that the visuomotor system operates in real time.

[1]  A Mack,et al.  Saccadic programming and perceived location. , 1981, Acta psychologica.

[2]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[3]  Paul Milgram,et al.  A spectacle-mounted liquid-crystal tachistoscope , 1987 .

[4]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of uncertainty and premovement visual information on manual aiming. , 1990, Canadian journal of psychology.

[5]  D. Elliott,et al.  A visual representation and the control of manual aiming movements. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[6]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[7]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[8]  L. Jakobson,et al.  Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  D. Elliott,et al.  The role of target information on manual-aiming bias , 1995, Psychological Research.

[10]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  M. Goodale,et al.  The objects of action and perception , 1998, Cognition.

[12]  J. Crawford,et al.  Gaze-Centered Remapping of Remembered Visual Space in an Open-Loop Pointing Task , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  M. Goodale,et al.  Frames of Reference for Perception and Action in the Human Visual System , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[15]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  David A. Westwood,et al.  Pantomimed actions may be controlled by the ventral visual stream , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  M. Goodale,et al.  The effects of delay on the kinematics of grasping , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  A. Milner,et al.  A paradoxical improvement of misreaching in optic ataxia: new evidence for two separate neural systems for visual localization , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Michael A. Khan,et al.  The Effect of Practice on Component Submovements is Dependent on the Availability of Visual Feedback , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  M. Goodale,et al.  Grasping after a Delay Shifts Size-Scaling from Absolute to Relative Metrics , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  M. Lemay,et al.  A distance effect in a manual aiming task to remembered targets: a test of three hypotheses , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  A. Milner,et al.  Grasping the past delay can improve visuomotor performance , 2001, Current Biology.

[24]  E A Roy,et al.  The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. , 2001, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[25]  W. Helsen,et al.  A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  M. Goodale,et al.  Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. , 2003, Spatial vision.