An Event-related Potential Study of Selective Auditory Attention in Children and Adults

In a dichotic listening paradigm, event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded to linguistic and nonlinguistic probe stimuli embedded in 2 different narrative contexts as they were either attended or unattended. In adults, the typical N1 attention effect was observed for both types of probes: Probes superimposed on the attended narrative elicited an enhanced negativity compared to the same probes when unattended. Overall, this sustained attention effect was greater over medial and left lateral sites, but was more posteriorly distributed and of longer duration for linguistic as compared to nonlinguistic probes. In contrast, in 6-to 8-year-old children the ERPs were morphologically dissimilar to those elicited in adults and children displayed a greater positivity to both types of probe stimuli when embedded in the attended as compared to the unattended narrative. Although both adults and children showed attention effects beginning at about 100 msec, only adults displayed left-lateralized attention effects and a distinct, posterior distribution for linguistic probes. These results suggest that the attentional networks indexed by this task continue to develop beyond the age of 8 years.

[1]  H. Holt,et al.  Book Review: Mother West Wind's Animal Friends , 1913 .

[2]  Ruth Stiles Gannett My Father's Dragon , 1948 .

[3]  E. Maccoby,et al.  Age trends in selective listening. , 1966, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[4]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[6]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Electrical Signs of Selective Attention in the Human Brain , 1973, Science.

[7]  A. Doyle Listening to distraction: a developmental study of selective attention. , 1973, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[8]  R. B. Freeman,et al.  The ontogeny of brain lateralization for speech and nonspeech stimuli , 1975, Brain and Language.

[9]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Auditory evoked potentials during selective listening to dichotic speech messages , 1976 .

[10]  J. Stamm,et al.  Auditory evoked potentials and selective attention in formerly hyperactive adolescent boys. , 1977, The American journal of psychiatry.

[11]  S. Hillyard,et al.  The division of attention and the human auditory evoked potential , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  G. Geffen,et al.  The Development of Auditory Strategies of Attention. , 1978 .

[13]  J. Hagen,et al.  The development of selective attention under distracting conditions. , 1978 .

[14]  K. Squires,et al.  Age-related variations in evoked potentials to auditory stimuli in normal human subjects. , 1978, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[15]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials and selective attention to acoustic and phonetic cues , 1978, Biological Psychology.

[16]  G. Geffen,et al.  Development of Three Strategies of Attention in Dichotic Monitoring. , 1979 .

[17]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Early selective-attention effects on the evoked potential: A critical review and reinterpretation , 1979, Biological Psychology.

[18]  G. Geffen,et al.  Development of Selective Listening and Hemispheric Asymmetry. , 1979 .

[19]  L. L. Elliott Performance of children aged 9 to 17 years on a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence material with controlled word predictability. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  M. Kinsbourne,et al.  Asymmetries of selective listening and attention switching in children. , 1980 .

[21]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Endogenous brain potentials associated with selective auditory attention. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[22]  J. Stamm,et al.  Evoked potential and behavioral signs of attentive dysfunctions in hyperactive boys. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[23]  R. Näätänen Processing negativity: an evoked-potential reflection of selective attention. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[24]  Risto N t nen Processing negativity: An evoked-potential reflection. , 1982 .

[25]  L. Kempe Handbook of Physiology. Section I. The Nervous System , 1982 .

[26]  D. Lane,et al.  The Development of Selective Attention. , 1982 .

[27]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related potentials during selective attention to speech sounds , 1983, Biological Psychology.

[28]  J. Stamm,et al.  Event-related potential and behavioral correlates of attention in reading retardation. , 1983, Journal of clinical neuropsychology.

[29]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials reveal similar attentional mechanisms during selective listening and shadowing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  P. Tallal,et al.  Attentional Capabilities of Reading-Impaired Children during Dichotic Presentation of Phonetic and Complex Nonphonetic Sounds , 1987, Cortex.

[31]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Evidence for effects of selective attention in the mid-latency range of the human auditory event-related potential. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[32]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Effects of sex, age, and forced attention on dichotic listening in children: A longitudinal study , 1987 .

[33]  J. Rohrbaugh,et al.  Current trends in event-related potential research , 1987 .

[34]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Dichotic listening and reading acquisition in children: a one-year follow-up. , 1987, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[35]  J. H. Daruna,et al.  Development of the late components of auditory brain potentials from early childhood to adulthood. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[36]  T. Picton,et al.  The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[37]  Daruna Jh,et al.  Development of the late components of auditory brain potentials from early childhood to adulthood. , 1987 .

[38]  R. Woodcock Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests-Revised , 1987 .

[39]  D. Molfese,et al.  Right-hemisphere responses from preschool children to temporal cues to speech and nonspeech materials: Electrophysiological correlates , 1988, Brain and Language.

[40]  A. Schell,et al.  Topographic study of auditory event-related potentials in normal boys and boys with attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[41]  L. Martín,et al.  Auditory event-related potentials in well-characterized groups of children. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[42]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Right hemisphere EEG sensitivity to speech , 1989, Brain and Language.

[43]  A. Schell,et al.  Ontogeny of selective attention effects on event-related potentials in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal boys , 1990, Biological Psychiatry.

[44]  D. Woods The physiological basis of selective attention: Implications of event-related potential studies. , 1990 .

[45]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Modulation of early auditory processing during selective listening to rapidly presented tones. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[46]  K. Alho,et al.  Brain potential signs of feature processing during auditory selective attention. , 1991, Neuroreport.

[47]  D. Lane,et al.  Auditory attention switching: a developmental study. , 1991, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[48]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. , 1992, Science.

[49]  T. Yamada,et al.  AUDITORY EVENT‐RELATED POTENTIALS AND REACTION TIME IN CHILDREN EVALUATION OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT , 1993, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[50]  F. Bloom,et al.  Modulation of early sensory processing in human auditory cortex during auditory selective attention. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[51]  H. Neville,et al.  Language Acquisition and Cerebral Specialization in 20-Month-Old Infants , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Selection of speech messages in free-field listening. , 1993, Neuroreport.

[53]  D. Woods,et al.  Feature processing during high-rate auditory selective attention , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[54]  C. F. Taub,et al.  Finding a Link between Selective Auditory Attention and Reading Problems in Young Children: A Preliminary Investigation , 1994, Perceptual and motor skills.

[55]  A. Schell,et al.  Preferential neural processing of attended stimuli in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal boys. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[56]  P. Korpilahti,et al.  Auditory ERP components and mismatch negativity in dysphasic children. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[57]  D. L. Ryan-Jones,et al.  Attentional modulation of the mismatch negativity elicited by frequency differences between binaurally presented tone bursts. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[58]  O. Salonen,et al.  Auditory Evoked Magnetic Fields to Tones and Pseudowords in Healthy Children and Adults , 1995, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[59]  D. Friedman,et al.  The development of selective attention as reflected by event-related brain potentials. , 1995, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[60]  I. Tonnquist-Uhlén,et al.  Topography of auditory evoked long-latency potentials in normal children, with particular reference to the N1 component. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[61]  C. Nelson,et al.  Age-related changes in the electrophysiological response to visual stimulus novelty: a topographical approach. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[62]  C. Barthélémy,et al.  Temporal prominence of auditory evoked potentials (N1 wave) in 4-8-year-old children. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[63]  P. Simos,et al.  Behavioral and Electrophysiological Indices of Voicing-Cue Discrimination: Laterality Patterns and Development , 1997, Brain and Language.

[64]  R. Oades,et al.  Development and topography of auditory event-related potentials (ERPs): mismatch and processing negativity in individuals 8-22 years of age. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[65]  N. Kraus,et al.  Developmental changes in P1 and N1 central auditory responses elicited by consonant-vowel syllables. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[66]  P. Teale,et al.  Developmental changes in refractoriness of the neuromagnetic M100 in children , 1998, Neuroreport.

[67]  M. Bryden,et al.  Auditory attentional shifts in reading-disabled students: quantification of attentional effectiveness by the Attentional Shift Index , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[68]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Interstimulus interval and auditory event-related potentials in children: evidence for multiple generators. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[69]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Spatial attention to central and peripheral auditory stimuli as indexed by event-related potentials. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[70]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Selective tuning of the left and right auditory cortices during spatially directed attention. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[71]  E. Yund,et al.  Human brain specialization for phonetic attention. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[72]  M. Taylor,et al.  Tracking the development of the N1 from age 3 to adulthood: an examination of speech and non-speech stimuli , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[73]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Attention and selection in the growing child: views derived from developmental psychophysiology , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[74]  S. Palva,et al.  Discrimination of Speech and of Complex Nonspeech Sounds of Different Temporal Structure in the Left and Right Cerebral Hemispheres , 2000, NeuroImage.

[75]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Effects of attention on dichotic listening: An 15O‐PET study , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[76]  R. Uwer,et al.  The development of auditory evoked dipole source activity from childhood to adulthood , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[77]  J. Eggermont,et al.  Maturation of human central auditory system activity: evidence from multi-channel evoked potentials , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[78]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Relations between the Neural Bases of Dynamic Auditory Processing and Phonological Processing: Evidence from fMRI , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[79]  H. Neville,et al.  A developmental investigation of ERP auditory rhyming effects , 2002 .

[80]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging of Speech Perception in Infants , 2002, Science.

[81]  H. Shibasaki,et al.  Maturational change of parallel auditory processing in school-aged children revealed by simultaneous recording of magnetic and electric cortical responses , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[82]  J. Mehler,et al.  Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[83]  C. Nelson,et al.  The X-Trials: Neural Correlates of an Inhibitory Control Task in Children and Adults , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[84]  E. Yund,et al.  Attentional modulation of human auditory cortex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[85]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Development of attentional networks in childhood , 2004, Neuropsychologia.