Assimilation and contrast: the two sides of specific interference between action and perception

Perception and action have long been treated as relatively independent and serial processes. More recent views, however, consider perception and action as relying on a common set of processes and/or representations. The present paper will focus on a variety of specific (content-based) perception–action interactions that have been taken as support for such views. In particular, the following aspects will be considered: direction of influence (perception on action vs. action on perception), temporal type (concurrent vs. non-concurrent), functional relation (related/unrelated), and type of movements (biological vs. non-biological). Different extant models of the perception-action interface are discussed and a classification schema proposed that tries to explain when contrast and when assimilation effects will arise.

[1]  A. Sanders Towards a model of stress and human performance. , 1983, Acta psychologica.

[2]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  J. Freyd,et al.  Apparent Motion of the Human Body , 1990 .

[5]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[6]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Predicting point-light actions in real-time , 2007, NeuroImage.

[7]  Corrado Sinigaglia,et al.  Perception in action. , 2007, Acta bio-medica : Atenei Parmensis.

[8]  J R Simon,et al.  Effect of ear stimulated on reaction time and movement time. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  Iseult A. M. Beets,et al.  Online action-to-perception transfer: Only percept-dependent action affects perception , 2010, Vision Research.

[10]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Action observation can prime visual object recognition , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Y. Paulignan,et al.  An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.

[12]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. , 2007, Human movement science.

[13]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action for perception: a motor-visual attentional effect. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  James Stanley,et al.  Effects of Agency on Movement Interference During Observation of a Moving Dot Stimulus , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Visual Search Is Modulated by Action Intentions , 2002, Psychological science.

[16]  N. Smyrnis,et al.  Perception action interaction: the oblique effect in the evolving trajectory of arm pointing movements , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  The Structure of Attentional Resources , 1980 .

[18]  Maggie Shiffrar,et al.  Walking perception by walking observers. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  P. Repp Action Can Affect Auditory , 2007 .

[20]  J. Ellis,et al.  The representation of delayed intentions: a prospective subject-performed task? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  B. Hommel,et al.  Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  J. Decety,et al.  Comparative analysis of actual and mental movement times in two graphic tasks , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[24]  D Kahneman,et al.  Perceptual Deficit during a Mental Task , 1967, Science.

[25]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility: Editors' introduction , 1997 .

[26]  W. Prinz,et al.  Perceptual resonance: action-induced modulation of perception , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[28]  R. Ellis,et al.  Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[29]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Hand action preparation influences the responses to hand pictures , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  M. Fahle,et al.  Grasping Visual Illusions: No Evidence for a Dissociation Between Perception and Action , 2000, Psychological science.

[31]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .

[32]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Selection-for-action in visual search. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[33]  Jochen Müsseler,et al.  How independent from action control is perception? An event-coding account for more equally ranked crosstalks , 1999 .

[34]  Uta Frith,et al.  Your Own Action Influences How You Perceive Another Person's Action , 2004, Current Biology.

[35]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Fitts's Law Holds for Action Perception , 2007, Psychological science.

[36]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  When do we simulate non-human agents? Dissociating communicative and non-communicative actions , 2010, Cognition.

[37]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[38]  B. Hommel,et al.  Intentional control of attention: action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions , 2007, Psychological research.

[39]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object , 2003, Nature.

[40]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  A contrast effect between the concurrent production and perception of movement directions , 2008 .

[41]  W. Prinz,et al.  Seeing while moving: Measuring the online influence of action on perception , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[42]  W. Prinz,et al.  Acting while perceiving: assimilation precedes contrast , 2008, Psychological research.

[43]  B. Repp,et al.  Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[44]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  A. Wohlschläger,et al.  Visual motion priming by invisible actions , 2000, Vision Research.

[47]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Dynamical explorations of compatibility in perception-action coupling , 1997 .

[49]  Bruno L. Giordano,et al.  Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect , 2006, Cognition.

[50]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  The mirror reflects both ways: Action influences perception of others , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[51]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[52]  Justin N. Wood Visual working memory for observed actions. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[53]  W. Prinz,et al.  What part of an action interferes with ongoing perception? , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[54]  L. Parsons Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[55]  A. Hamilton,et al.  Interference effect of observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion , 2007, Social neuroscience.

[56]  Iseult A. M. Beets,et al.  Nonvisual motor learning improves visual motion perception: evidence from violating the two-thirds power law. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  Blinded by headlights. , 2002, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[58]  G. Knoblich,et al.  The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[59]  R. Ward Independence and integration of perception and action: An introduction , 2002 .

[60]  B. Repp,et al.  Action Can Affect Auditory Perception , 2007, Psychological science.

[61]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .

[62]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  Interactions between perception and action in a reaction task with overlapping S-R assignments , 2001, Psychological research.

[63]  M. Giese,et al.  Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception , 2006, Current Biology.

[64]  W. Prinz,et al.  Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .

[65]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Action-Related Properties Shape Object Representations in the Ventral Stream , 2007, Neuron.

[66]  R. Wallace,et al.  S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[67]  Z. Pylyshyn Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. , 1999, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[68]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Spatiotemporal Tuning of Rapid Interactions between Visual-Motion Analysis and Reaching Movement , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[70]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  On interference effects in concurrent perception and action , 2009, Psychological research.

[71]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[72]  R. Miall,et al.  Performing hand actions assists the visual discrimination of similar hand postures , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[73]  Scott Glover,et al.  Visual illusions affect planning but not control , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[74]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  Working memory for language is not special: Evidence for an articulatory loop for novel stimuli , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[75]  T. Bachmann,et al.  Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (Advances in Psychology, Vol. 129) , 1999 .

[76]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Object manipulation and motion perception: evidence of an influence of action planning on visual processing. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[77]  D Kerzel,et al.  Visual short-term memory is influenced by haptic perception. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[78]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Motor facilitation following action observation: A behavioural study in prehensile action , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[79]  Christian Keysers,et al.  The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[80]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[81]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[82]  de Ritske Jong,et al.  Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .

[83]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Action-specific influences on distance perception: a role for motor simulation. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[84]  Bruno H Repp,et al.  Does an auditory distractor sequence affect self-paced tapping? , 2006, Acta psychologica.

[85]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Seeing the action: neuropsychological evidence for action-based effects on object selection , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[86]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Visuomotor Priming , 2001 .

[87]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[88]  A. Wohlschläger,et al.  Mental and manual rotation. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[89]  A. Greenwald,et al.  On doing two things at once: time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.