Electrophysiological evidence for the importance of interpersonalcuriosity

Interpersonal curiosity (IPC) is an important intrinsic motivation in social interaction, yet studies focused on its neural mechanism are rare. In a three-agent (self, other, or computer) interactive gambling task, we recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) to a cue stimuli indicating whether participants will be informed of their own, of another participant's or the computer's outcomes such that curiosity will be satisfied (CWS) or curiosity will not be satisfied (CWN). The results showed that relative to the CWS cue stimuli the CWN cue evoked a larger late positive component (LPC) between approximately 400 ms and 700 ms after cue onset in both the Self and Other conditions, but not in the Computer condition. Additionally, participants reported stronger curiosity in the Other's outcomes than in the Computer's outcomes. Most importantly, participants' subjective rating of curiosity was significantly correlated with the amplitude of the LPC elicited by the CWN cue. Furthermore, scores in the "curiosity about emotion" subscale of the IPC Scale was significantly correlated with the LPC amplitude when the participants learn they will not be informed of the Other's outcomes. We suggest that (1) interpersonal information is of great significance to individuals and IPC is an important social motivator, and (2) LPC amplitude is sensitive toIPC.

[1]  B. Renner Curiosity About People: The Development of a Social Curiosity Measure in Adults , 2006, Journal of personality assessment.

[2]  F. Fincham,et al.  Curiosity and Exploration: Facilitating Positive Subjective Experiences and Personal Growth Opportunities , 2004, Journal of personality assessment.

[3]  Jordan A. Litman,et al.  Dimensionality of interpersonal curiosity , 2007 .

[4]  Jordan A. Litman,et al.  The Measurement of Curiosity As a Feeling of Deprivation , 2004, Journal of personality assessment.

[5]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Modulation of activity in medial frontal and motor cortices during error observation , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Context dependence of the event-related brain potential associated with reward and punishment. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[7]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  Modulation of the brain activity in outcome evaluation by interpersonal relationship: An ERP study , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[9]  M. Junghöfer,et al.  The selective processing of briefly presented affective pictures: an ERP analysis. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[10]  Mark Snyder,et al.  Personality and social behavior , 1985 .

[11]  Francesco Versace,et al.  Affective picture perception: Emotion, context, and the late positive potential , 2008, Brain Research.

[12]  M. Underwood,et al.  A developmental investigation of social aggression among children. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[13]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  The Wick in the Candle of Learning , 2009, Psychological science.

[14]  G. Loewenstein The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation. , 1994 .

[15]  J. Panksepp,et al.  The neurobiology of positive emotions , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[16]  Rongjun Yu,et al.  Brain responses to outcomes of one's own and other's performance in a gambling task , 2006, Neuroreport.

[17]  C. Zink,et al.  Know Your Place: Neural Processing of Social Hierarchy in Humans , 2008, Neuron.

[18]  Albert Bandura,et al.  Observational learning as a function of symbolization and incentive set. , 1966 .

[19]  Stanley Schachter,et al.  The Psychology of Affiliation: Experimental Studies of the Sources of Gregariousness , 1959 .

[20]  Francesco Versace,et al.  Emotional imagery: Assessing pleasure and arousal in the brain's reward circuitry , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[21]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[22]  Jordan Litman Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: Wanting and liking new information , 2005 .

[23]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Gossip in Evolutionary Perspective , 2004 .

[24]  H. Fowler Curiosity and exploratory behavior , 1965 .

[25]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  Perceiving an opponent's loss: gender-related differences in the medial-frontal negativity. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[26]  Charles D. Spielberger,et al.  Curiosity and Exploratory Behavior , 2012 .

[27]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[28]  K. Fliessbach,et al.  Social Comparison Affects Reward-Related Brain Activity in the Human Ventral Striatum , 2007, Science.

[29]  D. Berlyne A theory of human curiosity. , 1954, British journal of psychology.

[30]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[31]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[32]  L. Festinger A Theory of Social Comparison Processes , 1954 .

[33]  B. Gold,et al.  Functional Dissociation in Frontal and Striatal Areas for Processing of Positive and Negative Reward Information , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Jun'ichi Katayama,et al.  Self-relevant criteria determine the evaluation of outcomes induced by others , 2008, Neuroreport.

[35]  Robin M. Kowalski,et al.  Behaving badly: Aversive behaviors in interpersonal relationships. , 2001 .

[36]  Peter Salovey,et al.  A Social Comparison Account of Gossip , 2004 .

[37]  R. L. Rosnow Rumor and gossip in interpersonal interaction and beyond: A social exchange perspective. , 2001 .

[38]  L. Nystrom,et al.  Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  The P300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation , 2009, Brain Research.

[40]  Dan Foti,et al.  Motivated and controlled attention to emotion: Time-course of the late positive potential , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[41]  Jordan A. Litman,et al.  The measurement of perceptual curiosity , 2004 .

[42]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  Whose loss is it? Human electrophysiological correlates of non-self reward processing , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[43]  Thane S. Pittman,et al.  Curiosity and Control: On the Determinants of the Search for Social Knowledge , 1981 .

[44]  B. Fredrickson What Good Are Positive Emotions? , 1998, Review of general psychology : journal of Division 1, of the American Psychological Association.

[45]  Alison L. Chasteen,et al.  Your mistakes are mine: Self-other overlap predicts neural response to observed errors , 2010 .

[46]  C. N. Cofer,et al.  Motivation: Theory and research. , 1964 .

[47]  Jordan A. Litman,et al.  The nature and measurement of sensory curiosity , 2005 .

[48]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[49]  D. Perrett,et al.  A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions , 1996, Nature.