Numbers within Our Hands: Modulation of Corticospinal Excitability of Hand Muscles during Numerical Judgment

Developmental and cross-cultural studies show that finger counting represents one of the basic number learning strategies. However, despite the ubiquity of such an embodied strategy, the issue of whether there is a neural link between numbers and fingers in adult, literate individuals remains debated. Here, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation to study changes of excitability of hand muscles of individuals performing a visual parity judgment task, a task not requiring counting, on Arabic numerals from 1 to 9. Although no modulation was observed for the left hand muscles, an increase in amplitude of motor-evoked potentials was found for the right hand muscles. This increase was specific for smaller numbers (1 to 4) as compared to larger numbers (6 to 9). These findings indicate a close relationship between hand/finger and numerical representations.

[1]  Martha Flanders,et al.  Muscular and postural synergies of the human hand. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  S. Boniface,et al.  Magnetic brain stimulation with a double coil: the importance of coil orientation. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain Signatures of Meaning Access in Action Word Recognition , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  T. Hines,et al.  An odd effect: Lengthened reaction times for judgments about odd digits , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[5]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  The perception of phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  K. Fuson Children's Counting and Concepts of Number , 1987 .

[7]  Xavier Seron,et al.  Finger–digit compatibility in Arabic numeral processing , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  B. Greenberg,et al.  What is transcranial magnetic stimulation? , 2003, The Harvard mental health letter.

[9]  Michael Andres,et al.  Actions, Words, and Numbers , 2008 .

[10]  A. Mechelli,et al.  Effect of word and syllable frequency on activation during lexical decision and reading aloud , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[11]  Emily B. Myers,et al.  An event-related fMRI investigation of phonological-lexical competition , 2004, Brain and Language.

[12]  Pierre Barrouillet,et al.  Predicting arithmetical achievement from neuro-psychological performance: a longitudinal study , 1998, Cognition.

[13]  Jean-Luc Velay,et al.  Visual presentation of single letters activates a premotor area involved in writing , 2003, NeuroImage.

[14]  Mauro Pesenti,et al.  Masked priming effect with canonical finger numeral configurations , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Masami Ishihara,et al.  Touch perception reveals the dominance of spatial over digital representation of numbers , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Where Mathematics Comes From , 2000 .

[17]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[18]  J. Grafman,et al.  Visualizing Cortical Activation during Mental Calculation with Functional MRI , 1996, NeuroImage.

[19]  P. A. Suresh,et al.  Developmental Gerstmann's syndrome: a distinct clinical entity of learning disabilities. , 2000, Pediatric neurology.

[20]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  What the teacher's hands tell the student's mind about math. , 1999 .

[21]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[22]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data , 2006, NeuroImage.

[23]  M. Delazer,et al.  Arithmetic Facts without Meaning , 1997, Cortex.

[24]  Michael Andres,et al.  Finger counting: The missing tool? , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[25]  P. Gordon Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.

[26]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.

[27]  P. Rossini,et al.  Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[28]  J. Gerstmann SYNDROME OF FINGER AGNOSIA, DISORIENTATION FOR RIGHT AND LEFT, AGRAPHIA AND ACALCULIA: LOCAL DIAGNOSTIC VALUE , 1940 .

[29]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  The Mathematical Brain , 1999 .

[30]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Language and the Origin of Numerical Concepts , 2004, Science.

[31]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Topographical Layout of Hand, Eye, Calculation, and Language-Related Areas in the Human Parietal Lobe , 2002, Neuron.

[32]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  B. Butterworth The development of arithmetical abilities. , 2005, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[34]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Number and language : how are they related ? , 2004 .

[35]  Susan M. Wagner,et al.  How our hands help us learn , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Arithmetic and the Brain This Review Comes from a Themed Issue on Cognitive Neuroscience Edited the Intraparietal Sulcus and Number Sense Number Sense in the Animal Brain , 2022 .

[37]  M. Alibali,et al.  Transitions in concept acquisition: using the hand to read the mind. , 1993, Psychological review.

[38]  R. J. Seitz,et al.  A fronto‐parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI‐study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[39]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral activations during number multiplication and comparison: a PET study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Klaus Willmes,et al.  Notational Modulation of the SNARC and the MARC (Linguistic Markedness of Response Codes) Effect , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[41]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  Changes in corticospinal motor excitability induced by non-motor linguistic tasks , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Emily B. Myers,et al.  An event-related fMRI investigation of phonological–lexical competition , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words , 1992, Cognition.

[44]  E. Warrington,et al.  The developmental Gerstmann syndrome. , 1968, Archives of neurology.

[45]  A. Kleinschmidt,et al.  A Supramodal Number Representation in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[46]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[47]  K. Poeck,et al.  Phantome nach Amputation und bei angeborenem Gliedmaßenmangel , 1969 .

[48]  Mauro PESENTI,et al.  Neuroanatomical substrates of Arabic number processing, numerical comparison and simple addition: A PET study. , 1998, NeuroImage.

[49]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic , 1997, Cortex.

[50]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[51]  Bruce W. Smith,et al.  The impact of affect and frequency on lexical decision: The role of the amygdala and inferior frontal cortex , 2006, NeuroImage.

[52]  E. Wassermann Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5-7, 1996. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[53]  A. Nieder Counting on neurons: the neurobiology of numerical competence , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[54]  E. Mayer,et al.  A pure case of Gerstmann syndrome with a subangular lesion. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[55]  M. Alibali,et al.  The function of gesture in learning to count: more than keeping track * , 1999 .

[56]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  The perception of phantom limbs , 1998 .

[57]  Liane Kaufmann,et al.  Dyscalculia: neuroscience and education , 2008, Educational research; a review for teachers and all concerned with progress in education.

[58]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Understanding dissociations in dyscalculia: a brain imaging study of the impact of number size on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate calculation. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[59]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Speech‐induced changes in corticospinal excitability , 1996, Annals of neurology.

[60]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[61]  K Poeck [Phantom limbs after amputation and in congenital missing limbs]. , 1969, Deutsche medizinische Wochenschrift.

[62]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The Organization of Brain Activations in Number Comparison: Event-Related Potentials and the Additive-Factors Method , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Dexterity with numbers: rTMS over left angular gyrus disrupts finger gnosis and number processing , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Differential Contributions of the Left and Right Inferior Parietal Lobules to Number Processing , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Approximate quantities and exact number words: dissociable systems , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[66]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[67]  C. Fiebach,et al.  fMRI Evidence for Dual Routes to the Mental Lexicon in Visual Word Recognition , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[68]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  Neural Correlates of Simple and Complex Mental Calculation , 2001, NeuroImage.

[69]  M. Posner,et al.  Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.