Characteristics of “anti” saccades in man

SummaryFour subjects — all made large numbers of Express saccades in the normal gap task — were instructed to make saccades in the direction opposite to the side where a visual stimulus appeared (“anti” task). Gap and overlap trials were used. Saccadic reaction time (SRT), velocity and amplitude of the corresponding eye movements were analysed and compared to those of saccades made in the normal task. The velocity of “anti saccades” was found to be slightly (up to 15%) but significantly slower in two subjects. The distributions of SRTs in normal gap tasks show a small group of anticipatory saccades (with SRT below 80 ms and slower velocities) followed by a group of saccades with fast reaction times between 80 ms and 120 ms (Express saccades) followed by another large group ranging up to 180 ms (regular saccades). In the gap anti task there are anticipatory saccades and saccades with SRTs above 100 ms; Express saccades are missing. The distribution of SRTs obtained in the overlap anti task was unimodal with a mean value of 231 ms as compared to 216 ms in the normal task. The introduction of the gap therefore clearly decreases the reaction times of the anti saccades. Control experiments show that the delay of anti saccades is not due to an interhemispheric transfer time but must be attributed to the saccade generating system taking more time to program a saccade to a position where no visual stimulus appears. These data are discussed as providing further evidence for the existence of a reflex-like pathway connecting the retina to the oculomotor nuclei mediating the Express saccade.

[1]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Dependence of saccadic eye-movements on stimulus luminance, and an effect of task , 1988, Vision Research.

[2]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task , 1980, Vision Research.

[3]  J. L. Conway,et al.  Deficits in eye movements following frontal eye-field and superior colliculus ablations. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  On plotting amplitude-transition functions for voluntary eye saccades , 1987, Vision Research.

[5]  Z. Kapoula,et al.  Evidence for a range effect in the saccadic system , 1985, Vision Research.

[6]  J. L. Conway,et al.  Effects of frontal eye field and superior colliculus ablations on eye movements. , 1979, Science.

[7]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  B. Fischer,et al.  Further observations on the occurrence of express-saccades in the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  B. Fischer,et al.  Human express saccades: effects of randomization and daily practice , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[12]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Role of striate cortex and superior colliculus in visual guidance of saccadic eye movements in monkeys. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  B. Fischer,et al.  Saccadic eye movements after extremely short reaction times in the monkey , 1983, Brain Research.

[14]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  The reduction of saccadic latency by prior offset of the fixation point: An analysis of the gap effect , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  S. Petersen,et al.  Saccade-related and visual activities in the pulvinar nuclei of the behaving rhesus monkey , 1986, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  H Deubel,et al.  Adaptive gain control of saccadic eye movements. , 1986, Human neurobiology.

[17]  B. Breitmeyer,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention revealed by saccadic eye movements , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  F. Ottes,et al.  Latency dependence of colour-based target vs nontarget discrimination by the saccadic system , 1985, Vision Research.

[19]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  The differentiation of visually guided and anticipatory saccades in gap and overlap paradigms , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  F. Ottes,et al.  Metrics of saccade responses to visual double stimuli: Two different modes , 1984, Vision Research.

[21]  J. V. Gisbergen,et al.  A parametric analysis of human saccades in different experimental paradigms , 1987, Vision Research.

[22]  A. C. Smit,et al.  A short-latency transition in saccade dynamics during square-wave tracking and its significance for the differentiation of visually-guided and predictive saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  J. Findlay Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements , 1982, Vision Research.

[24]  B Fischer,et al.  The preparation of visually guided saccades. , 1987, Reviews of physiology, biochemistry and pharmacology.

[25]  R. Douglas,et al.  Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  D. Frost,et al.  Different programming modes of human saccadic eye movements as a function of stimulus eccentricity: Indications of a functional subdivision of the visual field , 1976, Biological Cybernetics.

[27]  G M Gauthier,et al.  Two-dimensional eye movement monitor for clinical and laboratory recordings. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[28]  B. Fischer,et al.  Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.