The human orbitofrontal cortex monitors outcomes even when no reward is at stake

The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) processes the occurrence or omission of anticipated rewards, but clinical evidence suggests that it might serve as a generic outcome monitoring system, independent of tangible reward. In this positron emission tomography (PET) study, normal human subjects performed a series of tasks in which they simply had to predict behind which one of two colored rectangles a drawing of an object was hidden. While all tasks involved anticipation in that they had an expectation phase between the subject's prediction and the presentation of the outcome, they varied with regards to the uncertainty of outcome. No comment on the correctness of the prediction, no record of ongoing performance, and no reward, not even a score, was provided. Nonetheless, we found strong activation of the OFC: in comparison with a baseline task, the left anterior medial OFC showed activation in all conditions, indicating a basic role in anticipation; the left posterior OFC was activated in all tasks with some uncertainty of outcome, suggesting a role in the monitoring of outcomes; the right medial OFC showed activation exclusively during guessing. The data indicate a generic role of the human OFC, with some topical specificity, in the generation of hypotheses and processing of outcomes, independent of the presence of explicit reward.

[1]  A. Damasio Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. avon books , 1994 .

[2]  W. Schultz,et al.  Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[3]  M. Raichle,et al.  Anatomic Localization and Quantitative Analysis of Gradient Refocused Echo-Planar fMRI Susceptibility Artifacts , 1997, NeuroImage.

[4]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Functional Imaging of Neural Responses to Expectancy and Experience of Monetary Gains and Losses tasks with monetary payoffs , 2001 .

[5]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  T. Robbins,et al.  Choosing between Small, Likely Rewards and Large, Unlikely Rewards Activates Inferior and Orbital Prefrontal Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  A. Schnider,et al.  The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  M E Bellemann,et al.  Functional MR imaging of the prefrontal cortex: specific activation in a working memory task. , 1997, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[9]  Gregor Thut,et al.  Activation of the human brain by monetary reward , 1997, Neuroreport.

[10]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation , 2001, NeuroImage.

[11]  E. Rolls,et al.  Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Passingham,et al.  Active maintenance in prefrontal area 46 creates distractor-resistant memory , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Ptak,et al.  Recovery from spontaneous confabulations parallels recovery of temporal confusion in memory , 2000, Neurology.

[14]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR , 1986 .

[15]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[16]  A. Damasio,et al.  Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural Activity in the Human Brain Relating to Uncertainty and Arousal during Anticipation , 2001, Neuron.

[18]  J. Hobson,et al.  The prefrontal cortex in sleep , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  Joaquin M. Fuster,et al.  Single cell activity in ventral prefrontal cortex of behaving monkeys , 1981, Brain Research.

[20]  J. Parkinson,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of the Human Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex to Incentive Motivation and Goal Selection , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation , 1985, Neurology.

[22]  Elso Arruda,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: anatomy, physiology and neuropsychology of the frontal lobo , 1990 .

[23]  R. Elliott,et al.  Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates guessing , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  M Petrides,et al.  Orbitofrontal cortex: A key prefrontal region for encoding information. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[26]  C Burger,et al.  A JAVA environment for medical image data analysis: initial application for brain PET quantitation. , 1998, Medical informatics = Medecine et informatique.

[27]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[28]  M. Petrides,et al.  Differential activation of the human orbital, mid-ventrolateral, and mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during the processing of visual stimuli , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  J B Poline,et al.  The neural system that bridges reward and cognition in humans: An fMRI study , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  A R Damasio,et al.  Developmental consequences of childhood frontal lobe damage. , 1992, Archives of neurology.

[32]  Armin Schnider,et al.  Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of thought to ongoing reality , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[33]  R. J. Dolan,et al.  Differential neural response to positive and negative feedback in planning and guessing tasks , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  T. Robbins,et al.  Defining the Neural Mechanisms of Probabilistic Reversal Learning Using Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[36]  W. Schultz,et al.  Modifications of reward expectation-related neuronal activity during learning in primate orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Michael Petrides,et al.  Orbitofrontal Cortex and Memory Formation , 2002, Neuron.

[38]  S. Thorpe,et al.  The orbitofrontal cortex: Neuronal activity in the behaving monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  B H Price,et al.  The comportmental learning disabilities of early frontal lobe damage. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  Armin Schnider,et al.  Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  W. Schultz,et al.  Reward-related neuronal activity during go-nogo task performance in primate orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  Valerie Treyer,et al.  Selection of Currently Relevant Memories by the Human Posterior Medial Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Neural Responses during Anticipation of a Primary Taste Reward , 2002, Neuron.